Short-distance navigation in cephalopods: a review and synthesis
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Schiller. Delayed detour response in the octopus. , 1949, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[2] S. Katsanevakis,et al. Den ecology of Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797, on soft sediment: availability and types of shelter , 2004 .
[3] A. Darmaillacq,et al. Effects of dorsal and ventral vertical lobe electrolytic lesions on spatial learning and locomotor activity in Sepia officinalis. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.
[4] J. Mather. Daytime activity of juvenile Octopus vulgaris in Bermuda , 1988 .
[5] M. Lucero,et al. Behavioral responses to chemical stimulation of the olfactory organ in the squid Loligo opalescens , 1992 .
[6] Johanna B. Holm,et al. Octopuses (Octopus bimaculoides) and cuttlefishes (Sepia pharaonis, S. officinalis) can conditionally discriminate , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[7] N. L. Thomas,et al. On the architecture and function of cuttlefish bone , 1983 .
[8] J. A. Mather,et al. Foraging strategies and predation risk shape the natural history of juvenile Octopus vulgaris , 1991 .
[9] R. Hanlon,et al. Principal features of the mating system of a large spawning aggregation of the giant Australian cuttlefish Sepiaapama (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) , 2002 .
[10] E. Hartwick,et al. A removal experiment with Octopus dofleini (Wulker) , 1978 .
[11] M. Wells. DETOUR EXPERIMENTS WITH OCTOPUSES , 1964 .
[12] S. Adamo,et al. Cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis: Cephalopoda) hunting behavior and associative learning , 2004, Animal Cognition.
[13] R. O'dor,et al. The secret life of the giant Australian cuttlefish Sepia apama (Cephalopoda): Behaviour and energetics in nature revealed through radio acoustic positioning and telemetry (RAPT) , 2005 .
[14] N. Shashar,et al. Cuttlefish use polarization sensitivity in predation on silvery fish , 2000, Vision Research.
[15] J. Boal,et al. Distance chemoreception in the common cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis (Mollusca, Cephalopoda) , 1999 .
[16] B. Hochner,et al. A learning and memory area in the octopus brain manifests a vertebrate-like long-term potentiation. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] L. Regolin,et al. Perceptual and motivational aspects of detour behaviour in young chicks , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[18] Jennifer A. M.ather. Foraging, feeding and prey remains in middens of juvenile Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca: Cephalopoda) , 1991 .
[19] R. Hanlon,et al. Experimental evidence for spatial learning in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[20] J. Messenger,et al. A note on the ultrastructure of the Octopus olfactory organ , 1974, Cell and Tissue Research.
[21] A. Packard,et al. CEPHALOPODS AND FISH: THE LIMITS OF CONVERGENCE , 1972 .
[22] Achim Kempf,et al. On Path Integration On , 1996 .
[23] W. Barnes,et al. Mechanisms of homing in the fiddler crab Uca rapax 2. Information sources and frame of reference for a path integration system , 2003, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[24] L. Dickel,et al. The "prawn-in-the-tube" procedure in the cuttlefish: habituation or passive avoidance learning? , 2006, Learning & memory.
[25] Jennifer A. Mather,et al. Navigation by spatial memory and use of visual landmarks in octopuses , 1991, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[26] P. Lee,et al. Chemotaxis by Octopus maya Voss et Solis in a Y-maze , 1992 .
[27] O. N. Rood,et al. ON COLOR BLINDNESS. , 1898, Science.
[28] R. Wehner. Desert ant navigation: how miniature brains solve complex tasks , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[29] J. Young. Computation in the Learning System of Cephalopods. , 1991, The Biological bulletin.
[30] V. Braithwaite,et al. The influence of habitat stability on landmark use during spatial learning in the three-spined stickleback , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[31] J. Boal. A REVIEW OF SIMULTANEOUS VISUAL DISCRIMINATION AS A METHOD OF TRAINING OCTOPUSES , 1996, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[32] J. Young,et al. The statocysts of Octopus vulgaris , 1960, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[33] M F MOODY,et al. Discrimination of polarized light by octopus. , 1960, Nature.
[34] A. Darmaillacq,et al. BEHAVIOURAL PLASTICITY AND NEURAL CORRELATES IN ADULT CUTTLEFISH , 2006 .
[35] John L. Yarnall. Aspects of the behaviour of Octopus cyanea gray , 1969 .
[36] Roger T. Hanlon,et al. Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay , 2006, Vision Research.
[37] D. Dewsbury,et al. Octopus: Physiology and behaviour of an advanced invertebrate. , 1978 .
[38] J. Mather. Choice and competition: Their effects on occupancy of Shell Homes by Octopus joubini , 1982 .
[39] R. Hanlon,et al. Foraging and associated behavior by Octopus cyanea Gray, 1849 on a coral atoll, French Polynesia , 1997 .
[40] B. Budelmann. Cephalopod sense organs, nerves and the brain: Adaptations for high performance and life style. , 1995 .
[41] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[42] Ludovic Dickel,et al. Food imprinting, new evidence from the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis , 2006, Biology Letters.
[43] J. Young,et al. The statocyst of the squid Loligo , 1982 .
[44] A. Darmaillacq,et al. Rapid taste aversion learning in adult cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[45] N. Tinbergen. On aims and methods of Ethology , 2010 .
[46] F. Restle. Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. , 1957, Psychological review.
[47] J. Boal. Social recognition : A top down view of cephalopod behaviour , 2006 .
[48] C. Huffard,et al. Ethogram of Abdopus aculeatus (d'Orbigny, 1834) (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae): can behavioural characters inform octopodid taxomony and systematics? , 2007 .
[49] J. Mather. ‘Home’ choice and modification by juvenile Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca: Cephalopoda): specialized intelligence and tool use? , 1994 .
[50] M. F. Moody,et al. The Visual System of Octopus: (2) Discrimination of Polarized Light by Octopus , 1960, Nature.
[51] Brett M Gibson,et al. Place versus response learning revisited: tests of blocking on the radial maze. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.
[52] R. Ambrose. Shelter Utilization by the Molluscan Cephalopod Octopus bimaculatus , 1982 .
[53] H. Bleckmann,et al. A lateral line analogue in cephalopods: water waves generate microphonic potentials in the epidermal head lines ofSepia andLolliguncula , 1988, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[54] T. Collett,et al. Visual landmarks and route following in desert ants , 1992, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[55] J. Mather. Sand digging in Sepia officinalis: Assessment of a Cephalopod mollusc's "fixed" behavior pattern. , 1986 .
[56] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Detour behaviour in three species of birds: quails (Coturnix sp.), herring gulls (Larus cachinnans) and canaries (Serinus canaria) , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[57] M. Bitterman,et al. The octopus in the laboratory. Handling, maintenance, training , 1970 .
[58] L. Dickel,et al. Orientation in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis: response versus place learning , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[59] G. Biederman,et al. Social learning in invertebrates. , 1993, Science.
[60] R. Ambrose,et al. Den utilization and the movements of tagged Octopus dofleini , 1984 .
[61] A S Etienne,et al. Path integration in mammals and its interaction with visual landmarks. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[62] Marcia L. Spetch,et al. Mechanisms of landmark use in mammals and birds. , 1998 .
[63] S. Healy. Spatial representation in animals. , 1998 .
[64] Jennifer A. Mather,et al. Cognition in cephalopods , 1995 .
[65] J. Mather,et al. Activity and movement patterns of Octopus dofleini , 1985 .
[66] M. Wells,et al. A Cephalopod. (Book Reviews: Octopus. Physiology and Behaviour of an Advanced Invertebrate) , 1978 .
[67] R. O'dor,et al. The constraints on cephalopods: why squid aren't fish , 1986 .
[68] R. Hanlon,et al. Experimental evidence for spatial learning on octopuses (octopus bimaculoides). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.
[69] L. Dickel,et al. Effects of rearing conditions on sand digging efficiency in juvenile cuttlefish , 2004, Behavioural Processes.
[70] N. J. Marshall,et al. Colour-blind camouflage , 1996, Nature.
[71] P. Graziadei. Receptors in the Sucker of the Cuttlefish , 1964, Nature.
[72] G. Fiorito,et al. Observational Learning in Octopus vulgaris , 1992, Science.
[73] L. Dickel,et al. The effect of early experience on learning and memory in cuttlefish. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.
[74] L. Nadel,et al. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .
[75] John Zachary Young,et al. The Brains and Lives of Cephalopods , 2003 .
[76] L. Dickel,et al. Early experience and postembryonic maturation of body patterns in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.
[77] C. G. O'Brien. An Octopus , 1880, Nature.
[78] Nestor A. Schmajuk,et al. Purposive behavior and cognitive mapping: a neural network model , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.
[79] R. Hanlon,et al. Adaptive Coloration in Young Cuttlefish (Sepia Officinalis L.): The Morphology and Development of Body Patterns and Their Relation to Behaviour , 1988 .
[80] P. Boyle. Cephalopod life cycles vol ii comparative reviews , 1987 .