Use of interreflection and shadow for surface contact

The interaction of light with surfaces results in a number of lighting effects that may serve as valuable visual cues. Previous research on shadows has shown them to be effective in determining the three-dimensional (3-D) layout of a scene, but interreflections have been ignored as cues for spatial layout. Interreflections as well as shadows may help to disambiguate the 3-D layout of objects by providing information about an object’s contact with a surface. We generated computer images of a box on an extended textured ground plane that was either in contact with the ground or was slightly above the ground. Images were rendered for four conditions: (1) no shadow + no interreflection, (2) shadow only, (3) interreflection only, and (4) shadow + interreflection. A photometrically incorrect condition was also included. The participants rated the degree of contact for each image on a scale, which was used to generate receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves and a measure of sensitivity. In the images with no shadows or interreflections, the participants performed at chance levels. Interreflections, shadows, and a combination of interreflections and shadows all resulted in high sensitivity for judging object contact. More important, information from shadows and interreflections can be combined, resulting in near-perfect judgment of surface contact. Interreflections and shadows can be effective cues for object contact.

[1]  Takeo Kanade,et al.  Shape from interreflections , 1990, [1990] Proceedings Third International Conference on Computer Vision.

[2]  大野 義夫,et al.  Computer Graphics : Principles and Practice, 2nd edition, J.D. Foley, A.van Dam, S.K. Feiner, J.F. Hughes, Addison-Wesley, 1990 , 1991 .

[3]  Thomas Ertl,et al.  Computer Graphics - Principles and Practice, 3rd Edition , 2014 .

[4]  D Kersten,et al.  Moving Cast Shadows Induce Apparent Motion in Depth , 1997, Perception.

[5]  M. Carter Computer graphics: Principles and practice , 1997 .

[6]  James T. Kajiya,et al.  The rendering equation , 1998 .

[7]  David A. Forsyth,et al.  Reflections on Shading , 1991, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[8]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 1994 .

[9]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  Perception of Lightness and Illumination in a World of One Reflectance , 1984, Perception.

[10]  O. Sabouraud [Space perception]. , 1978, Revue d'oto-neuro-ophtalmologie.

[11]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Illusory motion from shadows , 1996, Nature.

[12]  Steven K. Feiner,et al.  Computer graphics: principles and practice (2nd ed.) , 1990 .

[13]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .

[14]  D. Knill,et al.  Geometry of shadows. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[15]  A. Hurlbert,et al.  Perception of three-dimensional shape influences colour perception through mutual illumination , 1999, Nature.

[16]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[17]  E. Adelson Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. , 1993, Science.

[18]  A Yonas,et al.  Development of Sensitivity to Information Provided by Cast Shadows in Pictures , 1978, Perception.

[19]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.

[20]  T. Marill Detection theory and psychophysics , 1956 .

[21]  Peter Shirley,et al.  Visual Glue , 1998 .