Object-based anisotropic mislocalization by retinotopic motion signals

The relative visual positions of briefly flashed stimuli are systematically modified in the presence of motion signals. We have recently shown that the perceived position of a spatially extended flash stimulus is anisotropically shifted toward a single convergent point back along the trajectory of a moving object without a significant change in the perceived shape of the flash [Watanabe, K., & Yokoi, K. (2006). Object-based anisotropies in the flash-lag effect. Psychological Science, 17, 728-735]. In the previous experiment, the moving stimulus moved in both retinotopic and environmental coordinates. In the present study, we examined whether the anisotropic mislocalization depends on retinotopic or object motion signals. When the retinal image of a moving stimulus was rendered stationary by smooth pursuit, the anisotropic pattern of mislocalization was not observed. In contrast, when the retinal image of a stationary stimulus was moved by eye movements, anisotropic mislocalization was observed, with the magnitude of the mislocalization comparable to that in the previous study. In both cases, there was little indication of shape distortion of the flash stimulus. These results demonstrate a clear case of object-based mislocalization by retinotopic motion signals; retinotopic--not object--motion signals distort the perceived positions of visual objects after the shape representations are established.

[1]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[2]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Shifts in perceived position of flashed stimuli by illusory object motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[4]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[5]  R. Nijhawan,et al.  Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition , 2001, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Freyd,et al.  A velocity effect for representational momentum , 1985 .

[8]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[10]  K. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Neuronal Processing Delays Are Compensated in the Sensorimotor Branch of the Visual System , 2003, Current Biology.

[11]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Perceived Shifts of Flashed Stimuli by Visible and Invisible Object Motion , 2003, Perception.

[12]  Sonja Stork,et al.  Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli: The Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag, and representational momentum , 2002 .

[13]  T. Vilis,et al.  Gaze-Centered Updating of Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  R Nijhawan,et al.  The Flash-Lag Phenomenon: Object Motion and Eye Movements , 2001, Perception.

[15]  Keiji Uchikawa,et al.  Apparent size of an object remains uncompressed during presaccadic compression of visual space , 2001, Vision Research.

[16]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[17]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Computational Theory and Cognition in Representational Momentum and Related Types of Displacement: A reply to Kerzel , 2006 .

[18]  M. Lappe,et al.  Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  Christopher A. Buneo,et al.  Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching , 2002, Nature.

[20]  S. Zeki A vision of the brain , 1993 .

[21]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[22]  D. Heeger,et al.  Retinotopy and Functional Subdivision of Human Areas MT and MST , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[24]  R. Andersen,et al.  Dorsal Premotor Neurons Encode the Relative Position of the Hand, Eye, and Goal during Reach Planning , 2006, Neuron.

[25]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Through the eye, slowly; Delays and localization errors in the visual system , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Y Tsal,et al.  Inattention magnifies perceived length: the attentional receptive field hypothesis. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.

[28]  I. Murakami,et al.  A flash-lag effect in random motion , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  B. Khurana,et al.  Perceptual organization of moving stimuli modulates the relative position of a visual flash , 2000 .

[30]  V. Lamme,et al.  The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  Katsumi Watanabe Visual grouping by motion precedes the relative localization between moving and flashed stimuli. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Behavioral reference frames for planning human reaching movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[34]  Eli Brenner,et al.  The role of uncertainty in the systematic spatial mislocalization of moving objects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Spatial distortions and processing latencies in the onset repulsion and Fröhlich effects , 2004, Vision Research.

[36]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Perceptual organization of moving stimuli modulates the flash-lag effect. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  R. Andersen,et al.  Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli , 1995, Nature.

[38]  J. Enns,et al.  Object Updating and the Flash-Lag Effect , 2004, Psychological science.

[39]  Kenji Yokoi,et al.  Object-Based Anisotropies in the Flash-Lag Effect , 2006, Psychological science.

[40]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[41]  D. Kerzel Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics , 2005 .

[42]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Separate simultaneous processing of egocentric and relative positions , 2000, Vision Research.

[43]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Mental extrapolation of target position is strongest with weak motion signals and motor responses , 2003, Vision Research.

[44]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Perisaccadic Mislocalization Orthogonal to Saccade Direction , 2004, Neuron.

[45]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.