Detection of silent intervals between noises activating different perceptual channels: some properties of "central" auditory gap detection.

This article describes four experiments on gap detection by normal listeners, with the general goal being to examine the consequences of using noises in different perceptual channels to delimit a silent temporal gap to be detected. In experiment 1, subjects were presented with pairs of narrow-band noise sequences. The leading element in each pair had a center frequency of 2 kHz and the trailing element's center frequency was parametrically varied. Gap detection thresholds became increasingly poor, sometimes by up to an order of magnitude, as the spectral disparity was increased between the noise bursts that marked the gap. These data suggested that gap-detection performance is impoverished when the underlying perceptual timing operation requires a comparison of activity in different perceptual channels rather than a discontinuity detection within a given channel. In experiment 2, we assessed the effect of leading-element duration in within-channel and between-channel gap detection tasks. Gap detection thresholds rose when the duration of the leading element was less than about 30 ms, but only in the between-channel case. In experiment 3, the gap-detection stimulus was redesigned so that we could probe the perceptual mechanisms that might be involved in stop consonant discrimination. The leading element was a wideband noise burst, and the trailing element was a 300-ms bandpassed noise centered on 1.0 kHz. The independent variable was the duration of the leading element, and the dependent variable was the smallest detectable gap between the elements. When the leading element was short in duration (5-10 ms), gap thresholds were close to 30 ms, which is close to the voice onset time that parses some voiced from unvoiced stop consonants. In experiment 4, the generality of the leading-element duration effect in between-channel gap detection was examined. Spectrally identical noises defining the leading and trailing edges of the gap were presented to the same or to different ears. There was a leading-element duration effect only for the between channel case. The mean gap threshold was again close to 30 ms for short leading-element durations. Taken together, the data suggest that gap detection requiring a temporal correlation of activity in different perceptual channels is a fundamentally different task to the discontinuity detection used to execute gap detection performance in the traditional, within-channel paradigm.

[1]  C. Formby,et al.  Detection of silent temporal gaps in narrow‐band noise stimuli having second‐formantlike properties of voiceless stop/vowel combinations , 1991 .

[2]  Walt Jesteadt,et al.  The relation between gap discrimination and auditory stream segregation , 1982 .

[3]  B C Moore,et al.  Gap detection as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  J D Miller,et al.  Speech perception by the chinchilla: identification function for synthetic VOT stimuli. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  J H Grose Gap detection in multiple narrow bands of noise as a function of spectral configuration. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  An investigation of categorical speech discrimination by rhesus monkeys , 1975 .

[7]  R. J. Salvi,et al.  Neural correlates of gap detection in auditory nerve fibers of the chinchilla , 1990, Hearing Research.

[8]  S Buus,et al.  Temporal gap detection in sensorineural and simulated hearing impairments. , 1984, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[9]  P. Fitzgibbons,et al.  Gap detection in normal and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  T. G. Forrest,et al.  Detection of silent temporal gaps in sinusoidal markers. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Selective adaptation of linguistic feature detectors , 1973 .

[13]  D M Green,et al.  Temporal gaps in noise and sinusoids. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  D G Sinex,et al.  Auditory-nerve fiber representation of temporal cues to voicing in word-medial stop consonants. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  J H Grose,et al.  Gap detection as a function of stimulus bandwidth with fixed high-frequency cutoff in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  D P Phillips,et al.  Word recognition performance in continuous and interrupted broad-band noise by normal-hearing and simulated hearing-impaired listeners. , 1995, The American journal of otology.

[17]  L H Carney,et al.  A temporal analysis of auditory-nerve fiber responses to spoken stop consonant-vowel syllables. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  B C Moore,et al.  Detection of temporal gaps in sinusoids: effects of frequency and level. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  B A Wright,et al.  Detection of unexpected tones with short and long durations. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  S J Norton,et al.  "Say-stay" identification and psychoacoustic performance of hearing-impaired listeners. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  D. Sinex,et al.  Average discharge rate representation of voice onset time in the chinchilla auditory nerve. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  B. Moore,et al.  Temporal window shape as a function of frequency and level. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  C. Formby,et al.  Modulation and gap detection for broadband and filtered noise signals. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  B C Moore,et al.  Gap detection with sinusoids and noise in normal, impaired, and electrically stimulated ears. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  M J Penner,et al.  Detection of temporal gaps in noise as a measure of the decay of auditory sensation. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  J H Grose,et al.  Gap detection for pairs of noise bands: effects of stimulus level and frequency separation. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  J H Grose,et al.  The detection of temporal gaps as a function of frequency region and absolute noise bandwidth. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Pierre L. Divenyi,et al.  Discrimination of time intervals marked by brief acoustic pulses of various intensities and spectra , 1977 .

[29]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Detection of temporal gaps within and between perceptual tonal groups , 1974 .

[30]  David R. Perrott,et al.  Temporal Resolution of Tonal Pulses , 1972 .

[31]  Phillips Dp,et al.  Impaired Word Recognition in Noise by Patients with Noise-Induced Cochlear Hearing-Loss - Contribution of Temporal Resolution Defect , 1994 .

[32]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  R. Plomp Rate of Decay of Auditory Sensation , 1964 .