Proprioceptive Localization of the Hand Changes When Skin Stretch around the Elbow Is Manipulated

Cutaneous information has been shown to influence proprioceptive position sense when subjects had to judge or match the posture of their limbs. In the present study, we tested whether cutaneous information also affects proprioceptive localization of the hand when moving it to a target. In an explorative study, we manipulated the skin stretch around the elbow by attaching elastic sports tape to one side of the arm. Subjects were asked to move the unseen manipulated arm to visually presented targets. We found that the tape induced a significant shift of the end-points of these hand movements. Surprisingly, this shift corresponded with an increase in elbow extension, irrespective of the side of the arm that was taped. A control experiment showed that this cannot be explained by how the skin stretches, because the skin near the elbow stretches to a similar extent on the inside and outside of the arm when the elbow angle increases and decreases, respectively. A second control experiment reproduced and extended the results of the main experiment for tape on the inside of the arm, and showed that the asymmetry was not just a consequence of the tape originally being applied slightly differently to the outside of the arm. However, the way in which the tape was applied does appear to matter, because applying the tape in the same way to the outside of the arm as to the inside of the arm influenced different subjects quite differently, suggesting that the relationship between skin stretch and sensed limb posture is quite complex. We conclude that the way the skin is stretched during a goal-directed movement provides information that helps guide the hand toward the target.

[1]  K. Kase,et al.  Clinical therapeutic applications of the Kinesio( !R)taping method , 2003 .

[2]  J. W. McChesney,et al.  Research article THE EFFECTS OF KINESIO TM TAPING ON PROPRIOCEPTION AT THE ANKLE , 2004 .

[3]  B. Edin Cutaneous afferents provide information about knee joint movements in humans , 2001, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  J. Abbs,et al.  Finger movement responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the dorsal skin of the human hand. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  M. Callaghan Role of ankle taping and bracing in the athlete. , 1997, British journal of sports medicine.

[7]  D. McCloskey Kinesthetic sensibility. , 1978, Physiological reviews.

[8]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Proprioception Is Robust under External Forces , 2013, PloS one.

[9]  S. Gandevia,et al.  The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. , 2012, Physiological reviews.

[10]  B. Edin,et al.  Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  E. Ribot-Ciscar,et al.  Cutaneous afferents provide a neuronal population vector that encodes the orientation of human ankle movements , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Errors in visuo-haptic and haptic-haptic location matching are stable over long periods of time. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Martha Flanders,et al.  Sensory control of target acquisition , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Peter Grigg,et al.  Peripheral Neural Mechanisms in Proprioception , 1994 .

[15]  Asir John Samuel,et al.  The Discrimination of Two-point Touch Sense for the Upper Extremity in Indian Adults - , 2013 .

[16]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Motor commands contribute to human position sense , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  I. A. Kuling,et al.  Torques do not influence proprioceptive localization of the hand , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Christopher A. Buneo,et al.  The Proprioceptive Map of the Arm Is Systematic and Stable, but Idiosyncratic , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  S C Gandevia,et al.  Impairment of human proprioception by high‐frequency cutaneous vibration , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  J.B.J. Smeets,et al.  A new binocular cue for absolute distance: Disparity relative to the most distant structure , 2010, Vision Research.

[21]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Haptic perception: A tutorial , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Proprioceptive Biases in Different Experimental Designs , 2014, EuroHaptics.

[23]  E. Brenner,et al.  Endpoints of arm movements to visual targets , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Sensory integration in the perception of movements at the human metacarpophalangeal joint , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  B. Edin Quantitative analysis of static strain sensitivity in human mechanoreceptors from hairy skin. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. F. Nolan,et al.  Two-point discrimination assessment in the upper limb in young adult men and women. , 1982, Physical therapy.

[27]  T. Findley,et al.  Deformations Experienced in the Human Skin, Adipose Tissue, and Fascia in Osteopathic Manipulative Medicine , 2014, The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association.

[28]  D. F. Collins,et al.  Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  D. Minciacchi,et al.  A little elastic for a better performance: kinesiotaping of the motor effector modulates neural mechanisms for rhythmic movements , 2014, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[30]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Bronks,et al.  The Effects of Kinesio™ Taping on Proprioception at the Ankle , 2004 .

[32]  J B J Smeets,et al.  Alignment to natural and imposed mismatches between the senses. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.