Understanding the Brain as an Endogenously Active Mechanism

Understanding the Brain as an Endogenously Active Mechanism William Bechtel (bill@mechanism.ucsd.edu) Department of Philosophy, University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0119 USA Adele Abrahamsen (aabrahamsen@ucsd.edu) Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093 USA The investigation of endogenous activity, though less in- fluential, has historical roots nearly as deep as those of the reactive approach. It was promoted by Thomas Graham Brown (1914), for example, who studied decerebrate and deafferented cats in Sherrington’s laboratory at Liverpool from 1910 to 1913. He found that the isolated spinal cord, even when not receiving inputs, generates patterns of activ- ity comparable to those exhibited during motor behavior elicited by stimuli. Brown’s emphasis on endogenous activ- ity initially was largely ignored (for discussion, see Stuart & Hultborn, 2008) but was revived several decades later when biologists recognized a class of neural circuits—central pattern generators—whose self-sustaining patterns of activ- ity generated rhythmic motor behavior even in the absence of sensory input. After Wilson and Wyman (1965) pio- neered this construct in their account of locust flight, others identified central pattern generators in the brain stem and spinal cord for walking, swimming, respiration, circulation, and other behaviors for which oscillatory control was cru- cial (Grillner, 2003). Endogenous activity has received far less attention from those studying sensory processing and central cognition rather than motor control, despite indica- tions of endogenous oscillatory activity in cerebral cortex using techniques ranging from single cell recording to EEG and fMRI. In the next section we describe highlights from this research and in the subsequent section briefly explore the implications for reconstruing how we understand cogni- tive activity. Most important, if the conception of the brain as endogenously active is taken seriously, it profoundly challenges the reactive perspective that has dominated much of cognitive science as well as neuroscience: stimuli or tasks must be regarded not as initiating activity in an inactive sys- tem, but rather as perturbing endogenous dynamic behavior. The slow pace at which these fields are achieving a change of perspective is unsurprising considering the his- tory of other sciences. Although Max Planck was exaggerat- ing when he said “A new scientific truth does not triumph by convincing its opponents . . . but rather because its oppo- nents eventually die . . .,” the considerable costs and uncer- tain benefits of change make it a tough sell. Uneven accep- tance of Einstein’s revolutionary proposals is a familiar ex- ample. Less remarked upon is the delayed impact of changes in the sciences on philosophy of science. For exam- ple, this young field (which did not even have a journal until 1934) did not exhibit acute concern with the epistemological foundations of science until it was confronted with Ein- Abstract Although a reactive framework has long been dominant in cognitive science and neuroscience, an alternative framework emphasizing dynamics and endogenous activity has recently gained prominence. We review some of the evidence for en- dogenous activity and consider the implications not only for understanding cognition but also for accounts of explanation offered by philosophers of science. Our recent characteriza- tion of dynamic mechanistic explanation emphasizes the co- ordination of accounts of mechanisms that identify parts and operations with computational models of their activity. These can, and should, be extended to incorporate attention to mechanisms that are not only active, but endogenously active. Keywords: philosophy of science; mechanistic explanation; dynamics; endogenous brain activity, resting state fMRI, brain default network Introduction Observe a living organism, from a bacterium to a fellow human being, and you see an endogenously active system. Introspect and you will observe, as did William James, a continual flow of thoughts. If pressed, most cognitive scien- tists will acknowledge that neural systems—from individual neurons to the brain as a whole—exhibit endogenous activ- ity. That is, some of the activity is internally (Greek endo) produced (German gennan); the causes and control of this activity is inside the system rather than reactive to inputs from outside the system. But cognitive scientists tend to disregard this when designing studies. Those in psychology present discrete stimuli in structured tasks designed to per- mit statistical analysis of the behavioral effects of independ- ent variables. Those in neuroscience, following the tradition of Charles Scott Sherrington (1923), commonly treat the brain as a reactive system in which sensory inputs initiate neural processing that results ultimately in motor responses. They may stimulate specific neurons or provide sensory inputs with specific properties so that recorded neural activ- ity can be analyzed in terms of responses to inputs. In both fields, variations in activity that cannot be associated with an input are treated as random fluctuations (noise). There is no doubt that this reactive framework in psychology and neuroscience has been enormously productive in identifying the parts, operations, and organization of the mechanisms responsible for cognition. It soon reaches its limits, though, in seeking accounts of the orchestrated functioning of those components: their dynamics and coordination in real time.

[1]  William Bechtel,et al.  Complex Biological Mechanisms : Cyclic , Oscillatory , and Autonomous , 2008 .

[2]  S. Grillner The motor infrastructure: from ion channels to neuronal networks , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  T. Brown On the nature of the fundamental activity of the nervous centres; together with an analysis of the conditioning of rhythmic activity in progression, and a theory of the evolution of function in the nervous system , 1914, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  P. Machamer,et al.  Thinking about Mechanisms , 2000, Philosophy of Science.

[6]  Ankoor S. Shah,et al.  An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  J. Kahn,et al.  Mental Mechanisms , 1965, Mental Health.

[8]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Symbiotic relationship between brain structure and dynamics , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[9]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  P. Thagard,et al.  Hot Thought: Mechanisms and Applications of Emotional Cognition , 2006 .

[12]  J. Antrobus,et al.  Mindwandering and cognitive structure. , 1970, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[14]  Alexa M. Morcom,et al.  Does the brain have a baseline? Why we should be resisting a rest , 2007, NeuroImage.

[15]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Pulin Gong,et al.  Evolution to a small-world network with chaotic units , 2004 .

[17]  E. Adrian,et al.  Thomas Graham Brown, 1882-1965 , 1966, Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society.

[18]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Rajita Sinha,et al.  Greater activation of the “default” brain regions predicts stop signal errors , 2007, NeuroImage.

[20]  B. Goodwin Oscillatory behavior in enzymatic control processes. , 1965, Advances in enzyme regulation.

[21]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  William Bechtel,et al.  Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and Localization as Strategies in Scientific Research , 2010 .

[23]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Neuronal Oscillations in Cortical Networks , 2004, Science.

[24]  M. Fox,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Antrobus,et al.  Section of Psychology: MINDWANDERING AND COGNITIVE STRUCTURE*, † , 1970 .

[26]  Duncan J. Watts,et al.  Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks , 1998, Nature.

[27]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Cliff Hooker Philosophy of complex systems , 2011 .

[29]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Interactions between membrane conductances underlying thalamocortical slow-wave oscillations. , 2003, Physiological reviews.

[30]  R. Edelman,et al.  Magnetic resonance imaging (2) , 1993, The New England journal of medicine.

[31]  William Bechtel,et al.  Dynamic mechanistic explanation: computational modeling of circadian rhythms as an exemplar for cognitive science. , 2010, Studies in history and philosophy of science.

[32]  W. Smith The Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907, Nature.

[33]  R. Wyman,et al.  MOTOR OUTPUT PATTERNS DURING RANDOM AND RHYTHMIC STIMULATION OF LOCUST THORACIC GANGLIA. , 1965, Biophysical journal.

[34]  W. Bechtel,et al.  Explanation: a mechanist alternative. , 2005, Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences.

[35]  R. Llinás The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: insights into central nervous system function. , 1988, Science.

[36]  Roel H. R. Deckers,et al.  Large-amplitude, spatially correlated fluctuations in BOLD fMRI signals during extended rest and early sleep stages. , 2006, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[37]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain , 2007, Nature.

[38]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.

[39]  R. K. Simpson Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .

[40]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar mri , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[41]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  The small world of the cerebral cortex , 2007, Neuroinformatics.

[42]  Hans Hultborn,et al.  Thomas Graham Brown (1882–1965), Anders Lundberg (1920–), and the neural control of stepping , 2008, Brain Research Reviews.