Motor memories in manipulation tasks are linked to contact goals between objects.
暂无分享,去创建一个
J Randall Flanagan | Jason P Gallivan | James N Ingram | James B Heald | Michael R McGarity-Shipley | Daniel Wolpert | James B. Heald | D. Wolpert | J. Flanagan | J. Ingram | J. Gallivan | Michael R. McGarity-Shipley
[1] D. Wolpert,et al. The effect of contextual cues on the encoding of motor memories , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[3] D. Wolpert,et al. The Value of the Follow-Through Derives from Motor Learning Depending on Future Actions , 2015, Current Biology.
[4] Miles C. Bowman,et al. Control strategies in object manipulation tasks , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[5] D. Nozaki,et al. Distinct Motor Plans Form and Retrieve Distinct Motor Memories for Physically Identical Movements , 2012, Current Biology.
[6] Stephen H Scott,et al. Limited transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual skills within the same limb , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[7] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Flexible Representations of Dynamics Are Used in Object Manipulation , 2008, Current Biology.
[8] T. Brashers-Krug,et al. Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[10] Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al. Short-term plasticity of the primary somatosensory cortex during tool use , 2004, Neuroreport.
[11] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[13] S. Scott,et al. Multi-compartment model can explain partial transfer of learning within the same limb between unimanual and bimanual reaching , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] Jörn Diedrichsen,et al. Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] P. Gribble,et al. Are there distinct neural representations of object and limb dynamics? , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] R A Scheidt,et al. Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] J R Flanagan,et al. The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] D. Wolpert,et al. Gone in 0.6 Seconds: The Encoding of Motor Memories Depends on Recent Sensorimotor States , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[19] S. Ichinose,et al. Extension of Corticocortical Afferents into the Anterior Bank of the Intraparietal Sulcus by Tool-use Training in Adult Monkeys , 2005 .
[20] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] T. Schormann,et al. Activation in the Ipsilateral Posterior Parietal Cortex during Tool Use: A PET Study , 2001, NeuroImage.
[22] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Motor learning of novel dynamics is not represented in a single global coordinate system: evaluation of mixed coordinate representations and local learning , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. A modular planar robotic manipulandum with end-point torque control , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[24] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[25] E. Todorov. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[26] David W Franklin,et al. Impedance control and internal model use during the initial stage of adaptation to novel dynamics in humans , 2005, The Journal of physiology.
[27] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[28] H. Onoe,et al. Functional Brain Mapping of Monkey Tool Use , 2001, NeuroImage.
[29] S. Scott. The computational and neural basis of voluntary motor control and planning , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[30] R. Johansson,et al. Eye–Hand Coordination in Object Manipulation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Fast But Fleeting: Adaptive Motor Learning Processes Associated with Aging and Cognitive Decline , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] D. Wolpert,et al. Failure to Consolidate the Consolidation Theory of Learning for Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] E. Bizzi,et al. Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.
[34] E Bizzi,et al. Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[35] J. Flanagan,et al. Neural Correlates of Internal-Model Loading , 2006, Current Biology.
[36] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Separate motor memories are formed when controlling different implicitly specified locations on a tool. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] M. Tanaka,et al. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[38] J. Randall Flanagan,et al. Multiple motor memories are learned to control different points on a tool , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.