Do Student Evaluations of University Reflect Inaccurate Beliefs or Actual Experience? A Relative Rank Model

It was shown that student satisfaction ratings are influenced by context in ways that have important theoretical and practical implications. Using questions from the UK's National Student Survey, the study examined whether and how students' expressed satisfaction with issues such as feedback promptness and instructor enthusiasm depends on the context of comparison (such as possibly inaccurate beliefs about the feedback promptness or enthusiasm experienced at other universities) that is evoked. Experiment 1 found strong effects of experimentally provided comparison context—for example, satisfaction with a given feedback time depended on the time's relative position within a context. Experiment 2 used a novel distribution-elicitation methodology to determine the prior beliefs of individual students about what happens in universities other than their own. It found that these beliefs vary widely and that students' satisfaction was predicted by how they believed their experience ranked within the distribution of others' experiences. A third study found that relative judgement principles also predicted students' intention to complain. An extended model was developed to show that purely rank-based principles of judgement can account for findings previously attributed to range effects. It was concluded that satisfaction ratings and quality of provision are different quantities, particularly when the implicit context of comparison includes beliefs about provision at other universities. Quality and satisfaction should be assessed separately, with objective measures (such as actual times to feedback), rather than subjective ratings (such as satisfaction with feedback promptness), being used to measure quality wherever practicable. © 2014 The Authors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

[1]  P. Spooren,et al.  Teacher professionalism and student evaluation of teaching: will better teachers receive higher ratings and will better students give higher ratings? , 2006 .

[2]  R. Carter Hill,et al.  Specifying Price Judgments with Range–Frequency Theory in Models of Brand Choice , 2009 .

[3]  A. Wood,et al.  An evolutionary based social rank explanation of why low income predicts mental distress: a 17 year cohort study of 30,000 people. , 2012, Journal of affective disorders.

[4]  A. Wood,et al.  Social norm influences on evaluations of the risks associated with alcohol consumption: applying the rank-based decision by sampling model to health judgments. , 2012, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[5]  Patricia M. McDonough,et al.  COLLEGE RANKINGS: Democratized College Knowledge for Whom? , 1998 .

[6]  Nira Hativa,et al.  University instructors' ratings profiles: Stability over time, and disciplinary differences , 1996 .

[7]  P. Abrami,et al.  Navigating student ratings of instruction. , 1997 .

[8]  A. Parducci Category judgment: a range-frequency model. , 1965, Psychological review.

[9]  P. Sedlmeier The role of scales in student ratings , 2006 .

[10]  Allen Parducci,et al.  Category rating scales: Effects of relative spacing and frequency , 1971 .

[11]  Andrew J. Martin,et al.  Use of student ratings to benchmark universities: Multilevel modeling of responses to the Australian Course Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) , 2011 .

[12]  J. Pettibone,et al.  Of gnomes and leprechauns: the recruitment of recent and categorical contexts in social judgment. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Michael B. Paulsen,et al.  Evaluating Teaching Performance , 2002 .

[14]  K. C. Johnson,et al.  Hot or Not: Do Professors Perceived as Physically Attractive Receive Higher Student Evaluations? , 2006, The Journal of general psychology.

[15]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  H. Marsh,et al.  Natural anchoring at the neutral point of category rating scales , 1978 .

[17]  A. Wood,et al.  Thanks, but I'm used to better: a relative rank model of gratitude. , 2011, Emotion.

[18]  Simon C. Moore,et al.  Money and Happiness , 2010, Psychological science.

[19]  Herbert W. Marsh,et al.  Effects of Grading Leniency and Low Workload on Students' Evaluations of Teaching: Popular Myth, Bias, Validity, or Innocent Bystanders? , 2000 .

[20]  Deborah Monique,et al.  Authors' addresses , 2004 .

[21]  Herbert W. Marsh,et al.  The Use of Students’ Evaluations and an Individually Structured Intervention to Enhance University Teaching Effectiveness , 1993 .

[22]  H. Marsh,et al.  Making students' evaluations of teaching effectiveness effective: The critical issues of validity, bias, and utility. , 1997 .

[23]  Douglas H. Wedell,et al.  Reference Price and Price Perceptions: A Comparison of Alternative Models , 2001 .

[24]  Christopher Y Olivola,et al.  Distributions of observed death tolls govern sensitivity to human fatalities , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Authors' Biographies , 2005 .

[26]  Douglas H. Wedell,et al.  Student Perceptions of Fair Grading: A Range-Frequency Analysis , 1989 .

[27]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  The Wisdom of Individuals: Exploring People's Knowledge About Everyday Events Using Iterated Learning , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[28]  Allen Parducci,et al.  Effects of context in judgments of sweetness and pleasantness , 1979 .

[29]  R. H. Smith,et al.  Intrapersonal and social comparison determinants of happiness: a range-frequency analysis. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Allen Parducci,et al.  Happiness, Pleasure, and Judgment: The Contextual Theory and Its Applications , 1995 .

[31]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Does the brain calculate value? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Ian Neath,et al.  How to Improve Your Teaching Evaluations without Improving Your Teaching , 1996 .

[33]  Chezy Ofir,et al.  In Search of Negative Customer Feedback: The Effect of Expecting to Evaluate on Satisfaction Evaluations , 2001 .

[34]  Dennis E. Clayson,et al.  Student Evaluations of Teaching: Are They Related to What Students Learn? , 2009 .

[35]  David Kjellberg Measuring Expectations , 1999, Identification Problems in the Social Sciences.

[36]  R. Calfee,et al.  Context effects in judgment: adaptation level as a function of the mean, midpoint, and median of the stimuli. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  A. Oswald,et al.  Does Wage Rank Affect Employees Well-Being? , 2005 .

[38]  H. Marsh,et al.  National Student Survey: are differences between universities and courses reliable and meaningful? , 2010 .

[39]  P. A. Cohen Student Ratings of Instruction and Student Achievement: A Meta-analysis of Multisection Validity Studies , 1981 .

[40]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Identification and bisection of temporal durations and tone frequencies: common models for temporal and nontemporal stimuli. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  H. Marsh Students ' Evaluations of University Teaching : Dimensionality , Reliability , Validity , Potential Biases , and Utility , 2005 .

[42]  Douglas H. Wedell,et al.  The Thick and the Thin of It: Contextual Effects in Body Perception , 2005 .

[43]  Barbara A. Mellers,et al.  Equity judgment: A revision of Aristotelian views. , 1982 .

[44]  D. H. Wedell,et al.  A formal analysis of ratings of physical attractiveness: Successive contrast and simultaneous assimilation , 1987 .

[45]  H. Marsh Students’ Evaluations of University Teaching: Dimensionality, Reliability, Validity, Potential Biases and Usefulness , 1984 .

[46]  J. Armstrong,et al.  Natural Learning in Higher Education , 2010 .