The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect measured by differential Pavlovian conditioning

We used a differential Pavlovian conditioning paradigm to measure tilt aftereffect (TAE) strength. Gabor patches, rotated clockwise and anticlockwise, were used as conditioned stimuli (CSs), one of which (CS+) was followed by the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), whereas the other (CS−) appeared alone. The UCS was an air puff delivered to the left eye. In addition to the CS+ and CS−, the vertical test patch was also presented for the clockwise and anticlockwise adapters. The vertical patch was not followed by the UCS. After participants acquired differential conditioning, eyeblink conditioned responses (CRs) were observed for the vertical patch when it appeared to be tilted in the same direction as the CS+ owing to the TAE. The effect was observed not only when the adapter and test stimuli were presented in the same retinotopic position but also when they were presented in the same spatiotopic position, although spatiotopic TAE was weak—it occurred approximately half as often as the full effect. Furthermore, spatiotopic TAE decayed as the time after saccades increased, but did not decay as the time before saccades increased. These results suggest that the time before the performance of saccadic eye movements is needed to compute the spatiotopic representation.

[1]  David C. Burr,et al.  Spatiotopic Coding of BOLD Signal in Human Visual Cortex Depends on Spatial Attention , 2011, PloS one.

[2]  N. Sobel,et al.  Humans can learn new information during sleep , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Norimichi Kitagawa,et al.  Hearing visual motion in depth , 2002, Nature.

[4]  Michael J. Morgan A bias-free measure of retinotopic tilt adaptation , 2014 .

[5]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Nonretinotopic visual processing in the brain , 2015, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Spatial invariance of visual receptive fields in parietal cortex neurons , 1997, Nature.

[7]  M. Morgan,et al.  Observers can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without losing sensitivity , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[8]  Standard delay eyeblink classical conditioning is independent of awareness. , 2002 .

[9]  Kerry Hourigan,et al.  Wake transition of a rolling sphere , 2011, J. Vis..

[10]  M. Webster,et al.  Adaptation to natural facial categories , 2002, Nature.

[11]  Diego E. Shalom,et al.  Classical conditioning in the vegetative and minimally conscious state , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Melcher Predictive remapping of visual features precedes saccadic eye movements , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Arash Afraz,et al.  Topography of the motion aftereffect with and without eye movements. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[14]  C. Koch The quest for consciousness : a neurobiological approach , 2004 .

[15]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Spatiotopic coding and remapping in humans , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  Nao Ninomiya,et al.  The 10th anniversary of journal of visualization , 2007, J. Vis..

[17]  J. Freeman,et al.  Neural circuitry and plasticity mechanisms underlying delay eyeblink conditioning. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[18]  D. Melcher Spatiotopic Transfer of Visual-Form Adaptation across Saccadic Eye Movements , 2005, Current Biology.

[19]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Spatiotopic neural representations develop slowly across saccades , 2013, Current Biology.

[20]  Y. Sugita,et al.  Surround-contingent motion aftereffect , 2015, Vision Research.

[21]  A. Mizuno,et al.  A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..

[22]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Phase-encoded fmri investigation of retinotopic remapping responses , 2010 .

[23]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  A reinvestigation of the reference frame of the tilt-adaptation aftereffect , 2013, Scientific Reports.

[24]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. , 1998, Science.

[25]  Katherine R. Storrs Are high-level aftereffects perceptual? , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[26]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[27]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Nonconscious fear is quickly acquired but swiftly forgotten , 2012, Current Biology.

[28]  P. P. Battaglini,et al.  Parietal neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordinates , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[30]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  Retinotopic encoding of the direction aftereffect , 2008, Vision Research.

[31]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[32]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Motion Aftereffects Transfer between Touch and Vision , 2009, Current Biology.

[33]  D. Burr,et al.  Spatiotopic selectivity of BOLD responses to visual motion in human area MT , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.