Cascaded processing in written naming: Evidence from the picture–picture interference paradigm

The issue of how information flows within the lexical system in written naming was investigated in five experiments. In Experiment 1, participants named target pictures that were accompanied by context pictures having phonologically and orthographically related or unrelated names (e.g., a picture of a “ball” superimposed on a picture of a “bed”). In Experiment 2, the related condition consisted of target and context pictures that shared the initial letter but not the initial sound. In both experiments, a facilitatory effect of relatedness was observed on the latencies. Experiment 3 tested whether phonology contributes to the facilitation effect found in the written latencies in Experiment 2 by using target and context pictures that shared the initial phoneme but not the initial grapheme. This experiment did not reveal any reliable difference between the related and unrelated conditions. In Experiments 4 and 5, control tasks were used to rule out a perceptual and conceptual account of the orthographic facilitation effect found in Experiment 2. The findings suggest that the recognition of an object leads to the activation of its name, and thus, that the activation within the lexical system in written-naming flows in a cascaded manner.

[1]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[2]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[3]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  SUBLEXICAL CONVERSION PROCEDURES AND THE INTERACTION OF PHONOLOGICAL AND ORTHOGRAPHIC LEXICAL FORMS , 1999 .

[4]  Hans Stadthagen-Gonzalez,et al.  Name–picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a large set of pictures , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  M. Weinrich,et al.  Further Evidence of a Dissociation between Output Phonological and Orthographic Lexicons: A Case Study , 1997 .

[6]  N. Geschwind,et al.  Problems in the Anatomical Understanding of the Aphasias , 1974 .

[7]  A. Caramazza,et al.  From graphemes to abstract letter shapes: levels of representation in written spelling. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[9]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[10]  Angela C. Jones,et al.  The Purpose of Lexical/Sublexical Interaction during Spelling: Further Evidence from Dysgraphia and Articulatory Suppression , 2004, Neurocase.

[11]  A. Young,et al.  Human Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2013 .

[12]  J G Snodgrass,et al.  Picture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  Boris New,et al.  Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE , 2001 .

[14]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .

[15]  Nancy J. Frishberg ARBITRARINESS AND ICONICITY: HISTORICAL CHANGE IN AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE , 1975 .

[16]  Antje S Meyer,et al.  Activation of distractor names in the picture-picture interference paradigm , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[17]  Gabriele Miceli,et al.  Disorders of single word processing , 2001, Journal of Neurology.

[18]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. , 1991 .

[19]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  A. Benton Contributions to clinical neuropsychology , 1969 .

[21]  Rita Capasso,et al.  Semantic Errors as Neuropsychological Evidence for the Independence and the Interaction of Orthographic and Phonological Word Forms , 1997 .

[22]  A Caramazza,et al.  Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: Implications for models of lexical access in language production , 2003 .

[24]  A. Roelofs Tracing attention and the activation flow of spoken word planning using eye movements. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Word frequency effects in speech production , 1994 .

[26]  Jan Rouke Kuipers,et al.  The limitations of cascading in the speech production system , 2009 .

[27]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Levels of representation, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect , 1990 .

[28]  A. Caramazza How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .

[29]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Handbook of Psycholinguistics , 2011 .

[30]  Brenda C Rapp,et al.  Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal About the Human Mind , 2001 .

[31]  Albert Costa,et al.  Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access , 2005 .

[32]  B. Rapp,et al.  Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.

[33]  Michele Miozzo,et al.  Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. , 1995 .

[35]  B. Rapp,et al.  Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production , 2007, Cognition.

[36]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[37]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Independence of Phonological and Orthographic Lexical Forms: Evidence from Aphasia , 1997 .

[38]  Pierre Mounoud,et al.  The influence of syllabic structure in handwriting and typing production , 1995 .

[39]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Semantic relatedness among objects promotes the activation of multiple phonological codes during object naming , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  M. Fayol,et al.  An Experimental Study of Lexical Access in the Writing and Naming of Isolated Words , 1998 .

[41]  Walburga Von Raffler-Engel,et al.  Language and cognitive styles : patterns of neurolinguistic and psycholinguistic development , 1982 .

[42]  W. Levelt,et al.  Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995) , 1996 .

[43]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The role of phonological and orthographic information in lexical selection , 2003, Brain and Language.

[44]  Victoria A. Fromkin,et al.  The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances , 1971 .

[45]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  Aleksandr R. Luria,et al.  Traumatic aphasia: Its syndromes, psychology and treatment , 1970 .

[47]  Brenda C Rapp,et al.  The spelling process. , 2002 .

[48]  G. Humphreys,et al.  An interactive activation approach to object processing: effects of structural similarity, name frequency, and task in normality and pathology. , 1995, Memory.

[49]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The status of double letters in Graphemic representations , 1996 .

[50]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Information flow in the mental lexicon during speech planning: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[51]  A. W. Ellis Reading, Writing and Dyslexia: A Cognitive Analysis , 1984 .

[52]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Cascade processes in picture identification , 1988 .

[53]  M. Damian,et al.  Impact of phonology on the generation of handwritten responses: Evidence from picture-word interference tasks , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[54]  A. W. Ellis Normality and pathology in cognitive functions , 1982 .

[55]  M. Fayol,et al.  Writing words from pictures: What representations are activated, and when? , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[56]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[57]  Peter A. Starreveld,et al.  On the Interpretation of Onsets of Auditory Context Effects in Word Production , 2000 .

[58]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Do perceived context pictures automatically activate their phonological code? , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[59]  Wido La Heij,et al.  THE LOCUS OF ORTHOGRAPHIC-PHONOLOGICAL FACILITATION : REPLY TO ROELOFS, MEYER, AND LEVELT (1996) , 1996 .

[60]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[61]  W. Levelt,et al.  Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form , 1994 .

[62]  Rosaleen A. McCarthy,et al.  Spelling and Writing , 1990 .

[63]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Autonomy of Lexical Orthography , 1997 .

[64]  Brenda Rapp,et al.  The interaction of lexical and sublexical information in spelling: What's the point? , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[65]  Christopher Barry,et al.  Are age-of-acquisition effects on object naming due simply to differences in object recognition? Comments on Levelt (2002) , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[66]  Alissa Melinger,et al.  Enhanced phonological facilitation and traces of concurrent word form activation in speech production: An object-naming study with multiple distractors , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[67]  R. Peereman,et al.  Do Phonological Codes Constrain the Selection of Orthographic Codes in Written Picture Naming , 2001 .

[68]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .