Cognitive Bias in Ambiguity Judgements: Using Computational Models to Dissect the Effects of Mild Mood Manipulation in Humans

Positive and negative moods can be treated as prior expectations over future delivery of rewards and punishments. This provides an inferential foundation for the cognitive (judgement) bias task, now widely-used for assessing affective states in non-human animals. In the task, information about affect is extracted from the optimistic or pessimistic manner in which participants resolve ambiguities in sensory input. Here, we report a novel variant of the task aimed at dissecting the effects of affect manipulations on perceptual and value computations for decision-making under ambiguity in humans. Participants were instructed to judge which way a Gabor patch (250ms presentation) was leaning. If the stimulus leant one way (e.g. left), pressing the REWard key yielded a monetary WIN whilst pressing the SAFE key failed to acquire the WIN. If it leant the other way (e.g. right), pressing the SAFE key avoided a LOSS whilst pressing the REWard key incurred the LOSS. The size (0–100 UK pence) of the offered WIN and threatened LOSS, and the ambiguity of the stimulus (vertical being completely ambiguous) were varied on a trial-by-trial basis, allowing us to investigate how decisions were affected by differing combinations of these factors. Half the subjects performed the task in a ‘Pleasantly’ decorated room and were given a gift (bag of sweets) prior to starting, whilst the other half were in a bare ‘Unpleasant’ room and were not given anything. Although these treatments had little effect on self-reported mood, they did lead to differences in decision-making. All subjects were risk averse under ambiguity, consistent with the notion of loss aversion. Analysis using a Bayesian decision model indicated that Unpleasant Room subjects were (‘pessimistically’) biased towards choosing the SAFE key under ambiguity, but also weighed WINS more heavily than LOSSes compared to Pleasant Room subjects. These apparently contradictory findings may be explained by the influence of affect on different processes underlying decision-making, and the task presented here offers opportunities for further dissecting such processes.

[1]  Zeb Kurth-Nelson,et al.  The modulation of savouring by prediction error and its effects on choice , 2016, eLife.

[2]  Y. Niv,et al.  Mood as Representation of Momentum , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  E. Bethell A “How-To” Guide for Designing Judgment Bias Studies to Assess Captive Animal Welfare , 2015, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.

[4]  P. Dayan,et al.  Depression: a decision-theoretic analysis. , 2015, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  Caroline Lee,et al.  Acute stress enhances sensitivity to a highly attractive food reward without affecting judgement bias in laying hens , 2015 .

[6]  L. Gygax The A to Z of statistics for testing cognitive judgement bias , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  Lisa F. Barrett,et al.  How does this make you feel? A comparison of four affect induction procedures , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[8]  E. Robinson,et al.  Modelling cognitive affective biases in major depressive disorder using rodents , 2014, British journal of pharmacology.

[9]  E. Fuchs,et al.  Chronic psychosocial stress makes rats more ‘pessimistic’ in the ambiguous-cue interpretation paradigm , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  C. Nederkoorn,et al.  Happy eating. The underestimated role of overeating in a positive mood , 2013, Appetite.

[11]  P. Dayan,et al.  Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis , 2013, Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders.

[12]  M. Wessa,et al.  Indirect assessment of an interpretation bias in humans: neurophysiological and behavioral correlates , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[13]  Daniel Nettle,et al.  The Evolutionary Origins of Mood and Its Disorders , 2012, Current Biology.

[14]  E. Gibson,et al.  The psychobiology of comfort eating: implications for neuropharmacological interventions , 2012, Behavioural pharmacology.

[15]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Go and no-go learning in reward and punishment: Interactions between affect and effect , 2012, NeuroImage.

[16]  E. Robinson,et al.  Evaluation of a novel translational task for assessing emotional biases in different species , 2011, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Serotonin Modulates the Effects of Pavlovian Aversive Predictions on Response Vigor , 2012, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[18]  A. Holmes,et al.  Cognitive bias in a non-human primate: husbandry procedures influence cognitive indicators of psychological well-being in captive rhesus macaques , 2012 .

[19]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Disentangling the Roles of Approach, Activation and Valence in Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[20]  A. Boissy,et al.  Administration of serotonin inhibitor p-Chlorophenylalanine induces pessimistic-like judgement bias in sheep , 2011, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[21]  M. Mendl,et al.  Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias , 2010, Current Biology.

[22]  M. Mendl,et al.  An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  I. Blanchette,et al.  The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning , 2010 .

[24]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. , 2010, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[25]  M. Mendl,et al.  Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals , 2009, Physiology & Behavior.

[26]  Richard M A Parker,et al.  Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms , 2009 .

[27]  M. Mendl,et al.  A spatial judgement task to determine background emotional state in laboratory rats, Rattus norvegicus , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  fMRI measurements of color in macaque and human. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[29]  M. Sahani,et al.  Implicit knowledge of visual uncertainty guides decisions with asymmetric outcomes. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[30]  Melissa Bateson,et al.  Larger, enriched cages are associated with 'optimistic' response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) , 2008 .

[31]  B. Spruijt,et al.  Tools to measure and improve animal welfare: reward-related behaviour , 2007, Animal Welfare.

[32]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk , 2007, Science.

[33]  H. Anisman,et al.  Stress, depression, and anhedonia: Caveats concerning animal models , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[34]  M. Mendl,et al.  Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[35]  M. Mendl,et al.  Animal behaviour: Cognitive bias and affective state , 2004, Nature.

[36]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[37]  K. Berridge,et al.  What is an unconscious emotion?(The case for unconscious "liking") , 2003, Cognition & emotion.

[38]  Alison J. Longley,et al.  Depression is an adaptation. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[39]  B. Spruijt,et al.  A concept of welfare based on reward evaluating mechanisms in the brain: anticipatory behaviour as an indicator for the state of reward systems. , 2001, Applied animal behaviour science.

[40]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[41]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[42]  P. Willner,et al.  “Depression” increases “craving” for sweet rewards in animal and human models of depression and craving , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[43]  Thomas E. Nygren,et al.  The Influence of Positive Affect on the Decision Rule in Risk Situations: Focus on Outcome (and Especially Avoidance of Loss) Rather Than Probability , 1996 .

[44]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  A. Isen,et al.  The influence of affect on categorization. , 1984 .

[46]  A. McElligott,et al.  The Use of Judgement Bias to Assess Welfare in Farm Livestock , 2015 .

[47]  I. Cuthill,et al.  Mood and the speed of decisions about anticipated resources and hazards , 2011 .

[48]  C. Grillon,et al.  Overgeneralization of conditioned fear as a pathogenic marker of panic disorder. , 2010, The American journal of psychiatry.

[49]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[50]  S. Mineka,et al.  Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[51]  G. A. Mendelsohn,et al.  Affect grid : A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal , 1989 .