A construction based analysis of child directed speech

The child directed speech of twelve English-speaking mothers was analyzed in terms of utterance-level constructions. First, the mothers’ utterances were categorized in terms of general constructional categories such as Wh-questions, copulas and transitives. Second, mothers’ utterances within these categories were further specified in terms of the initial words that framed the utterance, item-based phrases such asAre you. . . , I’ll . . . , It’s . . . , Let’s. . . , What did. . . . The findings were: (i) overall, only about 15% of all maternal utterances had SVO form (most were questions, imperatives, copulas, and fragments); (ii) 51% of all maternal utterances began with one of 52 item-based phrases, mostly consisting of two words or morphemes (45% began with one of just 17 words); and (iii) children used many of these same item-based phrases, in some cases at a rate that correlated highly with their own mother’s frequency of use. We suggest that analyses of adult–child linguistic interaction should take into account not just general constructional categories, but also the item-based constructions that adults and children use and the frequency with which they use them. © 2003 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved.

[1]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .

[2]  M. Maratsos,et al.  The internal language of children's syntax : The ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories , 1980 .

[3]  J. Pine Input and interaction in language acquisition: The language of primary caregivers , 1994 .

[4]  Patricia M. Clancy,et al.  The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of Preferred Argument Structure in Korean , 2003 .

[5]  G. Wells Learning through Interaction: The Study of Language Development , 1981 .

[6]  B. Richards,et al.  Input and interaction in language acquisition: Child-directed speech and influences on language acquisition: methodology and interpretation , 1994 .

[7]  E. Hoff-Ginsberg,et al.  Some contributions of mothers' speech to their children's syntactic growth , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[8]  D. Gentner,et al.  Similarity and the development of rules , 1998, Cognition.

[9]  M. Tomasello First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development , 1994 .

[10]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Child Language Data Exchange System: an update , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  M. Shatz,et al.  Linguistic input and the child's acquisition of language. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: the reduction of don't in English , 1999 .

[13]  V. Marchman,et al.  From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets , 1993, Cognition.

[14]  Ewa Dabrowska,et al.  From formula to schema: The acquisition of English questions , 2001 .

[15]  D. Slobin The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , 1987 .

[16]  J. Pine,et al.  Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[17]  Cynthia L Fisher,et al.  Structural limits on verb mapping: the role of abstract structure in 2.5‐year‐olds’ interpretations of novel verbs , 2002 .

[18]  Jill de Villiers,et al.  Learning how to use verbs: lexical coding and the influence of the input , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[19]  R. Gómez,et al.  11-month-Olds are sensitive to structure in an artificial grammar , 1999 .

[20]  E. Lieven Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children , 1994 .

[21]  C. Snow Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .

[22]  M. Braine Children's First Word Combinations. , 1976 .

[23]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[24]  A. Fernald Four-Month-Old Infants Prefer to Listen to Motherese" , 1985 .

[25]  Brian Richards,et al.  Language Development and Individual Differences: A Study of Auxiliary Verb Learning , 1990 .

[26]  Jean Christophe Verstraeh Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure , 2005 .

[27]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[28]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[29]  M. Maratsos Are actions to verbs as objects are to nouns? On the differential semantic bases of form, class, category , 1990 .

[30]  M. J. Farrar,et al.  Discourse and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[31]  Ruth Clark Performing without competence , 1974, Journal of Child Language.

[32]  Gordon Wells,et al.  Characteristics of adult speech which predict children's language development , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[33]  E. Markman,et al.  When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .

[34]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  Regular morphology and the lexicon. , 1995 .

[35]  J. Pine,et al.  Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category , 1997, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[36]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  M. Brent,et al.  The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development , 2001, Cognition.

[38]  Virginia Valian Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children , 1991, Cognition.

[39]  E. Newport MOTHERESE: THE SPEECH OF MOTHERS TO YOUNG CHILDREN. , 1975 .

[40]  Richard Timothy Beckwith Learnability and psychologically constrained grammars , 1988 .

[41]  W. Bruce Croft Intonation units and grammatical structure , 1995 .

[42]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[43]  H. Gleitman,et al.  The current status of the motherese hypothesis , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[44]  R. Langacker Foundations of cognitive grammar , 1983 .

[45]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[46]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[47]  C. Parisse,et al.  How children build their morphosyntax: the case of French , 2000, Journal of Child Language.

[48]  C Snow,et al.  Child language data exchange system , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[49]  S. Thompson,et al.  Transitivity, clause structure, and argument structure: Evidence from conversation , 2001 .

[50]  Anna L. Theakston,et al.  The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: an alternative account. , 2001, Journal of child language.

[51]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[52]  M. Tomasello,et al.  What preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders , 2001 .

[53]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Early syntactic creativity: a usage-based approach. , 2003, Journal of child language.

[54]  J. Pine,et al.  Reanalysing rote-learned phrases: individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[55]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Interplay of Function Morphemes and Prosody in Early Language , 1993 .

[56]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[57]  J. Pind The Discovery of Spoken Language, Peter W. Jusczyk (Ed.). MIT Press (1997), ISBN 0 262 10058 4 , 1997 .

[58]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Connectionism and language acquisition. , 2001 .

[59]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Modelling the Development of Dutch Optional Infinitives in MOSAIC , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[60]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[61]  Nick Chater,et al.  Distributional Information: A Powerful Cue for Acquiring Syntactic Categories , 1998, Cogn. Sci..

[62]  Lorraine Edith Kumpf,et al.  Preferred argument structure : grammar as architecture for function , 2003 .

[63]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[64]  D. Slobin Crosslinguistic Evidence for the Language-making Capacity , 1985 .

[65]  M. Tomasello,et al.  A tale of two theories: response to Fisher , 2002, Cognition.

[66]  J. Pine,et al.  Syntactic categories in the speech of young children: the case of the determiner , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[67]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[68]  D. Gentner,et al.  Language acquisition and conceptual development: Individuation, relativity, and early word learning , 2001 .

[69]  B. MacWhinney The Acquisition Of Morphophonology , 1978 .

[70]  M. Tomasello Regularity and Idiomaticity in Grammatical Constructions: The Case of Let Alone , 2003 .

[71]  W. Bruce Croft Radical Construction Grammar , 2001 .

[72]  K. Nelson,et al.  Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[73]  Anna L. Theakston,et al.  Going, going, gone: the acquisition of the verb ‘go’ , 2002, Journal of Child Language.

[74]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Signal to syntax : bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition , 1996 .

[75]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.