Naming morphologically complex pseudowords: A headstart for the root?

Several studies examining Italian have shown that morphemes are effective processing units in reading aloud. Faster naming times and greater accuracy have been found in the reading of pseudowords consisting of real root + real suffix, than for matched pseudowords not made up of such morphemes. The results of this study suggest that the root is of primary importance in the reading aloud of Italian pseudowords. Faster naming times were found both for real root + real suffix pseudowords and for real root + non-suffix pseudowords than for pseudowords which did not include any morpheme. This held true for a stimulus list consisting mostly of words (Experiment 1), and for one consisting of pseudowords only (Experiment 2). Real root + non-suffix pseudowords were read as fast as pseudowords that were fully parsable into morphemes (root + suffix), suggesting that a headstart to a morphemic route can be provided by the root only. However, root + non-suffix pseudowords were pronounced less accurately than root + suffix combinations, indicating that the identification of a root before initiating pronunciation may conflict with the full elaboration of vocal output. Experiment 3 investigated the roles of root and suffix. An effect of suffix on naming latencies was observed, but it was not as strong as the root effect. Taken together, the results suggest that roots and suffixes are accessed and activated in a cascaded manner during the reading aloud of Italian.

[1]  S. Andrews Morphological influences on lexical access: Lexical or nonlexical effects? , 1986 .

[2]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[3]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Morphological Representation as a Correlation Between form and Meaning , 2003 .

[4]  S. Lupker,et al.  Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .

[5]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Morpho-lexical Representations in Naming , 1997 .

[6]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Initial phoneme versus whole-word criterion to initiate pronunciation: Evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. , 1998 .

[7]  Lisa S Arduino,et al.  Lexical effects in left neglect dyslexia: A study in Italian patients , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[8]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  The processing of derived and inflected suffixed words during reading , 2000 .

[9]  A. Pollatsek,et al.  Reading Finnish compound words: eye fixations are affected by component morphemes. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  B. Ans,et al.  A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. , 1998, Psychological review.

[11]  Francesca Peressotti,et al.  Lexical Effects in Naming Pseudowords in Shallow Orthographies: Further Empirical Data , 1998 .

[12]  Cristina Burani,et al.  Morpholexical Access and Naming: The Semantic Interpretability of New Root–Suffix Combinations , 1999, Brain and Language.

[13]  F. Meunier,et al.  Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing , 2005 .

[14]  Alessandro Laudanna,et al.  Accessing and naming suffixed pseudo-words , 1997 .

[15]  Dan Chateau,et al.  Spelling–sound consistency effects in disyllabic word naming , 2003 .

[16]  Lisa S Arduino,et al.  Stress regularity or consistency? Reading aloud Italian polysyllables with different stress patterns , 2004, Brain and Language.

[17]  L. Colombo,et al.  The Influence of Age of Acquisition, Root Frequency, and Context Availability in Processing Nouns and Verbs , 2002, Brain and Language.

[18]  Robert Schreuder,et al.  Modeling morphological processing. , 1995 .

[19]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  C. Beauvillain,et al.  The Integration of Morphological and Whole-Word Form Information during Eye Fixations on Prefixed and Suffixed Words ☆ , 1996 .

[21]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[22]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Interactive-activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing , 1994 .

[23]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Naming multisyllabic words. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Robert Schreuder,et al.  The interpretation of isolated novel nominal compounds , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[25]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Lexical access and inflectional morphology , 1988, Cognition.

[26]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Lexical and Nonlexical Print-to-Sound Translation of Disyllabic Words and Nonwords , 2000 .

[27]  Cristina Burani,et al.  Neighborhood Effects on Nonword Visual Processing in a Language with Shallow Orthography , 2004, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[28]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Are long compound words identified serially via their constituents? Evidence from an eyemovement-contingent display change study , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Juan Segui,et al.  Reading aloud polysyllabic words , 2003 .

[30]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[31]  J. Segui,et al.  Morphological priming without morphological relationship , 2003 .

[32]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Serial Order in Planning the Production of Successive Morphemes of a Word , 1996 .

[33]  S. Kinoshita,et al.  Visual and Auditory Recognition of Prefixed Words , 1986 .

[34]  A. Pollatsek,et al.  The role of morphological constituents in reading Finnish compound words. , 2000 .

[35]  M. Taft Morphological Decomposition and the Reverse Base Frequency Effect , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  J. Hyönä,et al.  The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds , 2003 .

[37]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. , 1976 .

[38]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Morphology by itself in planning the production of spoken words , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Representation and processing of derived words , 1987 .

[40]  Barbara J. Juhasz,et al.  The effects of morphology on the processing of compound words: evidence from naming, lexical decisions and eye fixations. , 2003, British journal of psychology.

[41]  C. Burani,et al.  How Early Does Morpholexical Reading Develop in Readers of a Shallow Orthography? , 2002, Brain and Language.

[42]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.