The Co-Evolution of Number Concepts and Counting Words

Abstract Humans possess a number concept that differs from its predecessors in animal cognition in two crucial respects: (1) it is based on a numerical sequence whose elements are not confined to quantitative contexts, but can indicate cardinal/quantitative as well as ordinal and even nominal properties of empirical objects (e.g. ‘five buses’: cardinal; ‘the fifth bus’: ordinal; ‘the #5 bus’: nominal), and (2) it can involve recursion and, via recursion, discrete infinity. In contrast to that, the predecessors of numerical cognition that we find in animals and human infants rely on finite and iconic representations that are limited to cardinality and do not support a unified concept of number. In this paper, I argue that the way such a unified number concept could evolve in humans is via verbal sequences that are employed as numerical tools, that is, sequences of words whose elements are associated with empirical objects in number assignments. In particular, I show that a certain kind of number words, namely the counting sequences of natural languages, can be characterised as a central instance of verbal numerical tools. I describe a possible scenario for the emergence of such verbal numerical tools in human history that starts from iconic roots and that suggests that in a process of co-evolution, the gradual emergence of counting sequences and the development of an increasingly comprehensive number concept supported each other. On this account, it is language that opened the way for numerical cognition, suggesting that it is no accident that the same species that possesses the language faculty as a unique trait, should also be the one that developed a systematic concept of number.

[1]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[2]  James R. Hurford,et al.  Language and Number: The Emergence of a Cognitive System , 1987 .

[3]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[4]  T. Holyoke,et al.  Number Words and Number Symbols , 1978 .

[5]  I. Neath Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[6]  K. Absolon Dokumente und Beweise der Fahigkeiten des fossilen Menschen zu zahlen im mahrischen Palaolithikum , 1957 .

[7]  Geoffrey B. Saxe,et al.  Body parts as numerals: A developmental analysis of numeration among the Oksapmin in Papua New Guinea. , 1981 .

[8]  T. Deacon The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain , 1998 .

[9]  S. Carey Bootstrapping & the origin of concepts , 2004, Daedalus.

[10]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognition: Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee , 2000, Nature.

[12]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Child's Understanding of Number , 1979 .

[13]  H. Wiese Numbers, language, and the human mind , 2003 .

[14]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Macaque monkeys categorize images by their ordinal number , 2000, Nature.

[15]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Enumeration of collective entities by 5-month-old infants , 2002, Cognition.

[16]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Children's understanding of counting , 1990, Cognition.

[17]  Chris Donlan,et al.  The Development of Mathematical Skills , 2021 .

[18]  Herbert P. Ginsburg,et al.  The development of mathematical thinking , 1983 .

[19]  J. Leeuw,et al.  Abstract Measurement Theory. , 1986 .

[20]  A. Baddeley Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[21]  S E Antell,et al.  Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. , 1983, Child development.

[22]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Principles of Object Perception , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[23]  Susan Carey,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis , 2001 .

[24]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  The what and how of counting , 1990, Cognition.

[25]  S. Carey,et al.  Knowledge of Number: Its Evolution and Ontogeny , 1998, Science.

[26]  P. Bloom,et al.  Generativity within language and other cognitive domains , 1994, Cognition.

[27]  K. C. Fuson,et al.  The acquisition of early number word meanings: A conceptual analysis and review , 1983 .

[28]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Preschoolers' recognition of numerical equivalence: sequential sets. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Murdock,et al.  The serial position effect of free recall , 1962 .

[30]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[31]  H. Wiese Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development: the role of language , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Numerical competence in infants. , 1998 .

[33]  Tessei Kobayashi,et al.  Numerical competence in rats (Rattus norvegicus): Davis and Bradford (1986) extended. , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[34]  Ken Ramshøj Christensen The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain: , 2001 .

[35]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[36]  Jaap Van Brakel,et al.  Foundations of measurement , 1983 .

[37]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate-Inanimate Distinction as Examples , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  K. Wynn Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[40]  R. Gelman First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate‐Inanimate Distinction as Examples , 1990 .

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[42]  Susan Carey,et al.  What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge , 1999 .

[43]  James R. Hurford,et al.  The linguistic theory of numerals , 1975 .