Effects of Stroke on Ipsilesional End-Effector Kinematics in a Multi-Step Activity of Daily Living

Background: Stroke frequently impairs activities of daily living (ADL) and deteriorates the function of the contra- as well as the ipsilesional limbs. In order to analyze alterations of higher motor control unaffected by paresis or sensory loss, the kinematics of ipsilesional upper limb movements in patients with stroke has previously been analyzed during prehensile movements and simple tool use actions. By contrast, motion recording of multi-step ADL is rare and patient-control comparisons for movement kinematics are largely lacking. Especially in clinical research, objective quantification of complex externally valid tasks can improve the assessment of neurological impairments. Methods: In this preliminary study we employed three-dimensional motion recording and applied kinematic analysis in a multi-step ADL (tea-making). The trials were examined with respect to errors and sub-action structure, durations, path lengths (PLs), peak velocities, relative activity (RA) and smoothness. In order to check for specific burdens the sub-actions of the task were extracted and compared. To examine the feasibility of the approach, we determined the behavioral and kinematic metrics of the (ipsilesional) unimanual performance of seven chronic stroke patients (64a ± 11a, 3 with right/4 with left brain damage (LBD), 2 with signs of apraxia, variable severity of paresis) and compared the results with data of 14 neurologically healthy age-matched control participants (70a ± 7a). Results: T-tests revealed that while the quantity and structure of sub-actions of the task were similar. The analysis of end-effector kinematics was able to detect clear group differences in the associated parameters. Specifically, trial duration (TD) was increased (Cohen’s d = 1.77); the RA (Cohen’s d = 1.72) and the parameters of peak velocities (Cohen’s d = 1.49/1.97) were decreased in the patient group. Analysis of the task’s sub-actions repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA) revealed no impact of the different demands of the sub-actions on the relative performance of the patient group. Conclusion: The analyses revealed kinematic peculiarities in the performance with the ipsilesional hand. These deficits apparently arose from the cognitive demands like sequencing rather than motor constraints. End-effector kinematics proved as a sensitive method to detect and quantify aspects of disturbed multi-step ADL performance after stroke. If standardized, the examination and the analysis are quick and deliver objective data supporting clinical research.

[1]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J Hermsdörfer,et al.  The dependence of ipsilesional aiming deficits on task demands, lesioned hemisphere, and apraxia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  K. Sunnerhagen,et al.  Responsiveness of Upper Extremity Kinematic Measures and Clinical Improvement During the First Three Months After Stroke , 2013, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[4]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Pantomime of tool use depends on integrity of left inferior frontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  Myrna F Schwartz,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of everyday action and planning , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[6]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The case of the unfamiliar implement: Schema-based over-riding of semantic knowledge from objects in everyday action , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[7]  Charmayne M. L. Hughes,et al.  Segmenting Instrumented Activities of Daily Living (IADL) Using Kinematic and Sensor Technology for the Assessment of Limb Apraxia , 2013, HCI.

[8]  K M Heilman,et al.  Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Is the organisation of goal-directed action modality specific? A common temporal structure , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  R. Wood Task complexity: Definition of the construct , 1986 .

[11]  M. Johnson,et al.  Quantifying kinematics of purposeful movements to real, imagined, or absent functional objects: Implications for modelling trajectories for robot-assisted ADL tasks** , 2007, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[12]  David Howard,et al.  Movement variability in stroke patients and controls performing two upper limb functional tasks: a new assessment methodology , 2009, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[13]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome , 1998 .

[15]  Marion F Walker,et al.  Action errors and dressing disability after stroke: An ecological approach to neuropsychological assessment and intervention , 2006, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[16]  M. R. Patterson,et al.  FOURIER ANALYSIS OF UNEQUALLY SPACED DATA. , 1971 .

[17]  Laurel J Buxbaum,et al.  Naturalistic action production following right hemisphere stroke , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Katharina S Sunnerhagen,et al.  Three-dimensional kinematic motion analysis of a daily activity drinking from a glass: a pilot study , 2006, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[19]  Martin Lames,et al.  On the search for reliable performance indicators in game sports , 2007 .

[20]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Role of Semantic Knowledge and Working Memory in Everyday Tasks , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Manual and hemispheric asymmetries in the execution of actual and pantomimed prehension , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Diego Torricelli,et al.  Quantitative assessment based on kinematic measures of functional impairments during upper extremity movements: A review. , 2014, Clinical biomechanics.

[23]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  A Robust and Sensitive Metric for Quantifying Movement Smoothness , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[24]  B. Bussel,et al.  Evidence for Cognitive Processes Involved in the Control of Steady State of Walking in Healthy Subjects and after Cerebral Damage , 2005, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[25]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  The tool in the brain: apraxia in ADL. Behavioral and neurological correlates of apraxia in daily living , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[26]  K. Sunnerhagen,et al.  Movement Kinematics During a Drinking Task Are Associated With the Activity Capacity Level After Stroke , 2012, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[27]  Agnès Roby-Brami,et al.  Tool use kinematics across different modes of execution. Implications for action representation and apraxia , 2013, Cortex.

[28]  M. Boucart,et al.  Stroke and Action Slowing: Mechanisms, Determinants and Prognosis Value , 2010, Cerebrovascular Diseases.

[29]  G. Goldenberg Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[30]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  R T Knight,et al.  Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb apraxia. A kinematic analysis of aiming movements. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  Prehension With the Ipsilesional Hand After Unilateral Brain Damage , 1999, Cortex.

[33]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  James Gordon,et al.  Hemisphere Specific Impairments in Reach-to-Grasp Control After Stroke: Effects of Object Size , 2009, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[35]  A. Geurts,et al.  Upper-limb motor control in patients after stroke: attentional demands and the potential beneficial effects of arm support. , 2013, Human movement science.

[36]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[37]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  The complexity of the relationship between neuropsychological deficits and impairment in everyday tasks after stroke , 2015, Brain and behavior.

[38]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Quantifying the quality of hand movement in stroke patients through three-dimensional curvature , 2011, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[39]  K M Heilman,et al.  Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia. Neural representations of learned movement. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  K M Heilman,et al.  Ecological implications of limb apraxia: Evidence from mealtime behavior , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[41]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Left Parietal Regions Are Critical for Adaptive Visuomotor Control , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Won-Kyung Song,et al.  Kinematic analysis of upper extremity movement during drinking in hemiplegic subjects. , 2014, Clinical biomechanics.

[43]  Analysis of Eye Movements, Kinematics and Dynamic Aspects of Performance during Activities of Daily Living in Stroke Patients , 2014 .

[44]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[45]  K. Sunnerhagen,et al.  Kinematic Variables Quantifying Upper-Extremity Performance After Stroke During Reaching and Drinking From a Glass , 2011, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[46]  Pratik K. Mutha,et al.  Coordination deficits in ideomotor apraxia during visually targeted reaching reflect impaired visuomotor transformations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Systematic assessment of apraxia and functional predictions from the Birmingham Cognitive Screen , 2012, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.