Are Cognitive Symptoms of Schizophrenia Mediated by Abnormalities in Emotional Arousal?

ABSTRACT We tested 28 individuals with schizophrenia (SZ) and 16 healthy individuals on a test of logical reasoning and “cognitive gating,” defined as the ability to discriminate between relevant and irrelevant information in confirming or disconfirming a given belief. The Logical Reasoning and Cognitive Gating Task tests both processes under neutral and affect-laden conditions. This is done by presenting formally identical constructs using benign and emotionally arousing language. When separated by symptom profiles, we found statistically significant differences for performance and arousal response between patients with delusions, patients with formal thought disorder, and patients with neither delusions nor formal thought disorder, as well as between patients and healthy controls. When analyzed by error type, we found that nearly all errors by delusional patients were caused by overly restrictive information choice, a pattern that may be related to a delusional patient's tendency to “jump to conclusions” on Bayesian probabilistic tasks. This is in contrast to patients with formal thought disorder, whose low performance resulted also from overly extensive information choice. The tendencies towards restriction were exacerbated by arousal, which is consistent with studies on cognition and arousal in healthy individuals. After briefly examining research on emotional arousal and SZ, and the interaction between emotional arousal and restriction of perceptual cues in healthy individuals, we conclude by suggesting a model which accounts for the distinctive cognitive characteristics of delusional patients by their possessing distinct vulnerabilities to emotional arousal. Specifically, these results suggest the possibility that delusional patients process information in a manner that is essentially intact. However, delusional patients may possess an acute vulnerability to emotional arousal that might cause delusional individuals to behave cognitively as if they were healthy individuals under significantly more severe forms of stress.

[1]  P. Delespaul,et al.  The context of delusional experiences in the daily life of patients with schizophrenia , 2001, Psychological Medicine.

[2]  S. Grossberg The imbalanced brain: from normal behavior to schizophrenia , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  H. Sackeim,et al.  Logical Processing, Affect, and Delusional Thought in Schizophrenia , 2000, Harvard review of psychiatry.

[4]  M. Phillips,et al.  An Examination of Hypervigilance for External Threat in Individuals with Generalized Anxiety Disorder and Individuals with Persecutory Delusions Using Visual Scan Paths , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[5]  S. Ikemoto,et al.  Increased sensitivity to stress in spontaneous recurrence of methamphetamine psychosis: noradrenergic hyperactivity with contribution from dopaminergic hyperactivity. , 2000, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[6]  E. D. Haan,et al.  Laterality effects in selective attention to threat after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at the prefrontal cortex in female subjects , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Changes in adult brain and behavior caused by neonatal limbic damage: implications for the etiology of schizophrenia , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  Joseph E. LeDoux,et al.  How danger is encoded: towards a systems, cellular, and computational understanding of cognitive-emotional interactions , 1999 .

[9]  Jimmy D Bell,et al.  Changes in Auditory Selective Attention and Event-Related Potentials Following Oral Administration of D-amphetamine in Humans , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[10]  P. Garety,et al.  Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. , 1999, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[11]  P. Ayton,et al.  Reasoning biases in delusion-prone individuals. , 1998, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[12]  K. Nuechterlein,et al.  Electrodermal activity as a prodromal sign in schizophrenia , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[13]  Yoshinosuke Henmi Prodromal Symptoms of Relapse in Schizophrenic Outpatients: Retrospective and Prospective Study , 1993, The Japanese journal of psychiatry and neurology.

[14]  R. Tandon,et al.  Dexamethasone suppression test in schizophrenia: Relationship to symptomatology, ventricular enlargement, and outcome , 1991, Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  S. Wessely,et al.  Reasoning in Deluded Schizophrenic and Paranoid Patients: Biases in Performance on a Probabilistic Inference Task , 1991, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[16]  R. Sapolsky,et al.  Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged glucocorticoid exposure in primates , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  V. Yeragani The Incidence of Abnormal Dexamethasone Suppression in Schizophrenia: A Review and a Meta-Analytic Comparison with the Incidence in Normal Controls , 1990, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.

[18]  K. Nuechterlein,et al.  A prospective study of stressful life events and schizophrenic relapse. , 1989, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  P. Garety,et al.  Probabilistic Judgements in Deluded and Non-Deluded Subjects , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  J. Obiols The Clinical Roots of the Schizophrenia Concept: Translations of Seminal European Contributions on Schizophrenia , 1988 .

[21]  G. Sedvall,et al.  Dexamethasone suppression test in schizophrenic patients before and during neuroleptic treatment , 1986, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[22]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Magical ideation as an indicator of schizotypy. , 1983, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[23]  John H. Gruzelier,et al.  An analysis of the skin conductance orienting response in samples of American, British, and German schizophrenics , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[24]  A. Baddeley Selective attention and performance in dangerous environments. , 1972, British journal of psychology.

[25]  J. Gruzelier,et al.  SKIN CONDUCTANCE ORIENTING ACTIVITY IN A HETEROGENEOUS SAMPLE OF SCHIZOPHRENICS: Possible Evidence of Limbic Dysfunction , 1972, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[26]  G. R. J. Hockey Effect of Loud Noise on Attentional Selectivity , 1970 .

[27]  J. Easterbrook The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. , 1959, Psychological review.

[28]  A. Bursill The Restriction of Peripheral Vision during Exposure to Hot and Humid Conditions , 1958 .

[29]  F. C. Rockett Speed of form recognition as a function of stimulus factors and test anxiety. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[30]  A. Combs,et al.  The effect of the perception of mild degrees of threat on performance. , 1952, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[31]  R. Murray,et al.  Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[32]  S. Jönsson,et al.  Pyramidal neuron size in the hippocampus of schizophrenics correlates with total cell count and degree of cell disarray , 1999, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[33]  W. Gaebel,et al.  Predictors of relapse and rehospitalization in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. , 1998, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[34]  K. Nuechterlein,et al.  Electrodermal anomalies in recent-onset schizophrenia: relationships to symptoms and prognosis. , 1992, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[35]  K. Nuechterlein,et al.  Developmental Processes in Schizophrenic Disorders: longitudinal studies of vulnerability and stress. , 1992, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[36]  D. Klein,et al.  Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia--Lifetime Version modified for the study of anxiety disorders (SADS-LA): rationale and conceptual development. , 1986, Journal of psychiatric research.

[37]  S. Bacon,et al.  Arousal and the range of cue utilization. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  D. M. Cornsheet,et al.  Use of cues in the visual periphery under conditions of arousal. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  Richard P. Kern,et al.  Experimental studies of psychological stress in man. , 1962 .

[40]  S. Arieti Schizophrenia: The Manifest Symptomatology, the Psychodynamic and Formal Mechanisms , 1959 .

[41]  J. Bruner,et al.  Breadth of learning as a function of drive level and mechanization. , 1955, Psychological review.