Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence.

The development of theory of mind use was investigated by giving a computerized task to 177 female participants divided into five age groups: Child I (7.3-9.7 years); Child II (9.8-11.4); Adolescent I (11.5-13.9); Adolescent II (14.0-17.7); Adults (19.1-27.5). Participants viewed a set of shelves containing objects, which they were instructed to move by a 'director' who could see some but not all of the objects. Correct interpretation of critical instructions required participants to use the director's perspective and only move objects that the director could see. In a control condition, participants were asked to ignore objects in slots with a grey background. Accuracy improved similarly in both conditions between Child I and Adolescent II. However, while performance of the Adolescent II and Adult groups did not differ in the control condition, the Adolescent II group made more errors than the adults in the experimental condition. These results suggest that theory of mind use improves between late adolescence and adulthood. Thus, while theory of mind tasks are passed by age 4, these data indicate that the interaction between theory of mind and executive functions continues to develop in late adolescence.

[1]  Deanna Kuhn,et al.  The Importance of Learning About Knowing: Creating a Foundation for Development of Intellectual Values , 2009 .

[2]  D. Barr,et al.  Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[3]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Evidence of Perspective-Taking Constraints in Children's On-Line Reference Resolution , 2002, Psychological science.

[4]  D. Sperber,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Attribution of Beliefs by 13-Month-Old Infants , 2022 .

[5]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[6]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction , 2004 .

[7]  T. Q. Irigaray,et al.  Intellectual abilities in Alzheimer's disease patients: Contributions from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) , 2010 .

[8]  D. Barr,et al.  Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension , 2000, Psychological science.

[9]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[10]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[11]  M. Chandler,et al.  Relativism and stations of epistemic doubt. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[12]  Ian A. Apperly,et al.  The cost of thinking about false beliefs: Evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task , 2008, Cognition.

[13]  J. Flavell,et al.  Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. , 1981 .

[14]  M. Reiff,et al.  Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. , 2003 .

[15]  Vicki A. Anderson,et al.  Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[16]  Paul Bloom,et al.  The Curse of Knowledge in Reasoning About False Beliefs , 2007, Psychological science.

[17]  C. Frith,et al.  Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[18]  T. Ohnishi,et al.  Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence , 2007, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[19]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[20]  Jacob Cohen Measurement Educational and Psychological Educational and Psychological Measurement Eta-squared and Partial Eta-squared in Fixed Factor Anova Designs Educational and Psychological Measurement Additional Services and Information For , 2022 .

[21]  E. Robinson,et al.  Adolescents’ and adults’ views about the evidential basis for beliefs: Relativism and determinism re-examined , 1998 .

[22]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children , 2022 .

[25]  N. Epley,et al.  Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  H. Wimmer,et al.  “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children ☆ , 1985 .

[27]  E. Robinson,et al.  Contamination in reasoning about false belief: an instance of realist bias in adults but not children , 1996, Cognition.

[28]  Sunny Shin,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs ? , 2005 .

[29]  S. Blakemore The social brain in adolescence , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  B. Sodian,et al.  Now I see it but you don't: 14-month-olds can represent another person's visual perspective. , 2007, Developmental science.

[31]  Cecil R. Reynolds,et al.  A Model of the Development of Frontal Lobe Functioning: Findings From a Meta-Analysis , 2005, Applied neuropsychology.

[32]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Understanding children's and adults' limitations in mental state reasoning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.