Successful homing of magnet-carrying white-chinned petrels released in the open sea

Abstract During the incubation period, white-chinned petrels, Procellaria aequinoctialis , repeatedly make foraging trips of several thousand kilometres in the open ocean from their small isolated breeding island. To test whether these birds rely on geomagnetic information to home, we displaced five individuals and released them in the open sea 300–360 km from home. They were prevented both from using route-based information during the passive displacement from the breeding island to the release site, and from using the geomagnetic field at the release site and on their way home (by carrying a mobile magnet on the head). They were equipped with satellite transmitters to record their homing movements. The five petrels homed efficiently (in 17–32 h) along paths that were on average slightly less than twice the homing distance. This showed that, at least when within a few hundred kilometres of home, white-chinned petrels are able to home successfully by relying only on nongeomagnetic site-dependent information. Copyright 2003 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd on behalf of The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.

[1]  Wallraff The magnetic map of homing pigeons: an evergreen phantom , 1999, Journal of theoretical biology.

[2]  T. Roper Olfaction in birds , 1999 .

[3]  M. Valvo,et al.  Homing of Cory's shearwaters (Calonectris diomedea) carrying magnets , 1991 .

[4]  Donald R. Griffin,et al.  Homing Experiments with Leach's Petrels , 1940 .

[5]  P. Jouventin,et al.  Olfactory Behavior of Foraging Procellariiforms , 1994 .

[6]  Wallraff,et al.  Seven theses on pigeon homing deduced from empirical findings , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[7]  Peter Kareiva,et al.  Dimethyl sulphide as a foraging cue for Antarctic Procellariiform seabirds , 1995, Nature.

[8]  F. Papi,et al.  Olfactory navigation in birds , 1990, Experientia.

[9]  S. Benhamou,et al.  Homing in pelagic birds: a pilot experiment with white-chinned petrels released in the open sea , 2003, Behavioural Processes.

[10]  G. Hays,et al.  Testing the navigational abilities of ocean migrants: displacement experiments on green sea turtles (Chelonia mydas) , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[11]  H. Weimerskirch,et al.  Exploitation of distant Antarctic waters and close shelf-break waters by white-chinned petrels rearing chicks , 2000 .

[12]  Dnn,et al.  The Behaviour, Population Biology and Physiology of the Petrels , 1996 .

[13]  P. A. Prince,et al.  Foraging white-chinned petrels Procellaria aequinoctialis at risk: from the tropics to Antarctica , 1999 .

[14]  W. Wiltschko,et al.  Magnetic orientation in birds , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[15]  H. G. Wallraff Navigation by homing pigeons: updated perspective , 2001 .

[16]  Magnetic Orientation and Celestial Cues in Migratory Orientation , 1990 .

[17]  B. McConnell,et al.  Movements and foraging areas of naïve, recently weaned southern elephant seal pups , 2002 .

[18]  G. Nevitt,et al.  Olfactory foraging by Antarctic procellariiform seabirds: life at high Reynolds numbers. , 2000, The Biological bulletin.

[19]  S. Åkesson,et al.  OCEANIC NAVIGATION : ARE THERE ANY FEASIBLE GEOMAGNETIC BI-COORDINATE COMBINATIONS FOR ALBATROSSES? , 1998 .

[20]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  On systems of reference involved in spatial memory , 1997, Behavioural Processes.

[21]  H. G. Wallraff,et al.  Spatial gradients in ratios of atmospheric trace gases : a study stimulated by experiments on bird navigation , 2000 .

[22]  H. Weimerskirch,et al.  Could osmotaxis explain the ability of blue petrels to return to their burrows at night? , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.

[23]  G. Nevitt Foraging by Seabirds on an Olfactory Landscape , 1999, American Scientist.

[24]  S. Benhamou,et al.  Orientation in “Featureless” Environments: The Extreme Case of Pelagic Birds , 2003 .

[25]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[26]  W. Keeton,et al.  Magnets interfere with pigeon homing. , 1971, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  W. Ian Reilly Magnetic position determination by homing pigeons? , 2002, Journal of theoretical biology.

[28]  H. Weimerskirch,et al.  Magnetic cues: are they important in Black-browed Albatross Diomedea melanophris orientation? , 2002 .

[29]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Distinguishing between elementary orientation mechanisms by means of path analysis , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  P. Luschi,et al.  Influence of emotional factors on the initial orientation of pigeons , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  Pierre Jouventin,et al.  Foraging Strategy of Wandering Albatrosses Through The Breeding Season: A Study Using Satellite Telemetry , 1993 .

[32]  P. Luschi,et al.  Pigeon homing: evidence against reliance on magnetic information picked up en route to release sites , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Vincent Bretagnolle,et al.  Smelling home: a good solution for burrow-finding in nocturnal petrels? , 2002, The Journal of experimental biology.

[34]  W. Wiltschko,et al.  Magnetic orientation and celestial cues in migratory orientation , 1990, Experientia.

[35]  P. Jouventin,et al.  Influence of Breeding Success on Fidelity in Long-Lived Birds: An Experimental Study , 1999 .

[36]  W. Reilly Magnetic position determination by homing pigeons , 1999 .

[37]  T. Piersma,et al.  Proceedings of the 22nd International Ornithological Congress , 1999 .

[38]  H. Weimerskirch,et al.  Satellite tracking of Wandering albatrosses , 1990, Nature.

[39]  D. Costa,et al.  Fast and fuel efficient? Optimal use of wind by flying albatrosses , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.