Perceptual completion in newborn human infants.

Despite decades of studies of human infants, a still open question concerns the role of visual experience in the development of the ability to perceive complete shapes over partial occlusion. Previous studies show that newborns fail to manifest this ability, either because they lack the visual experience required for perceptual completion or because they fail to detect the pattern of motion. To distinguish these possibilities, newborns' perception of a center-occluded object was tested, using stroboscopic motion. Infants (mean age of 72 hr) perceived the object as a connected unit, providing the first evidence that the newborn is capable of filling in gaps in the visible surface layout when the relevant visual information can be detected by his or her immature visual system.

[1]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Newborn infant's perception of partly occluded objects , 1996 .

[2]  Gentaro Taga,et al.  Hemodynamic responses to visual stimulation in occipital and frontal cortex of newborn infants: a near-infrared optical topography study. , 2003, Early human development.

[3]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Synchronous change and perception of object unity: evidence from adults and infants , 1999, Cognition.

[4]  A. Norcia,et al.  The development of motion sensitivity during the first year of life , 1994, Vision Research.

[5]  Alan Slater,et al.  Perception of object unity in chicks: A comparison with the human infant☆ , 1996 .

[6]  A. Norcia,et al.  Anomalous motion VEPs in infants and in infantile esotropia. , 1991, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[7]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Infants' perception of object unity in translating and rotating displays. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[8]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[9]  G. Bronson The accurate calibration of infants' scanning records. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  J Wattam-Bell,et al.  Visual motion processing in one-month-old infants: Habituation experiments , 1996, Vision Research.

[11]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Development of basic visual functions , 1989 .

[12]  D. Maurer,et al.  Influence of a central stimulus on infants' visual fields , 1997 .

[13]  Louise Hainline,et al.  The retina of the newborn human infant. , 1982, Science.

[14]  The gap effect in newborns , 1999 .

[15]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  F. D. Horowitz,et al.  An Infant Control Procedure for Studying Infant Visual Fixations. , 1972 .

[17]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  The role of three-dimensional depth cues in infants' perception of partly occluded objects , 1994 .

[18]  R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al.  Development of infants' sensitivity for slow and rapid motions , 1985 .

[19]  L. Regolin,et al.  Object and spatial representations in detour problems by chicks , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Scott P. Johnson Theories of Development of the Object Concept , 2008 .

[21]  L. Regolin,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  David M. Regal,et al.  Development of critical flicker frequency in human infants , 1981, Vision Research.

[23]  Mark H. Johnson Cortical Maturation and the Development of Visual Attention in Early Infancy , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  P. Kellman,et al.  A theory of visual interpolation in object perception , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Perception of object unity in 2-month-old infants. , 1995 .

[26]  E. Spelke,et al.  Nativism, empiricism, and the development of knowledge. , 1998 .

[27]  E. Valenza,et al.  The Role of Perceptual Skills in Newborns' Perception of Partly Occluded Objects. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[28]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Infants' Detection of Shearing Motion in Random-Dot Displays. , 1992 .

[29]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Perception of object unity in young infants: The roles of motion, depth, and orientation , 1996 .

[30]  I ROCK,et al.  Perception of Stroboscopic Movement: Evidence for Its Innate Basis , 1965, Science.

[31]  S. Vehrs,et al.  A Test of Visual Responses in the Newborn , 1970, Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology.

[32]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Early perception-action coupling: Eye movements and the development of object perception. , 2000 .

[33]  Alan Slater,et al.  Newborn and older infants' perception of partly occluded objects☆ , 1990 .

[34]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  ARTICLE WITH PEER COMMENTARIES AND RESPONSE Learning to perceive object unity: a connectionist account , 2002 .

[35]  Detection of stimulus motion in 5-month-old infants. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  John Wattam-Bell,et al.  Development of motion-specific cortical responses in infancy , 1991, Vision Research.

[37]  E. Hartmann Perceptual Development: Visual, Auditory, and Speech Perception in Infancy , 1999 .

[38]  J. Dannemiller,et al.  The detection of slow stimulus movement in 2- to 5-month-olds. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[39]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  José E. Náñez,et al.  Young infant's perception of object unity in two-dimensional displays , 1995 .