The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience

art may employ ‘supernormal’ stimuli to excite form areas in the brain more strongly than natural stimuli. Second, we suggest that grouping is a very basic principle. The different extrastriate visual areas may have evolved specifically to extract correlations in different domains (e.g. form, depth, colour), and discovering and linking multiple features (‘grouping’) into unitary clusters — objects — is facilitated and reinforced by direct connections from these areas to limbic structures. In general, when object-like entities are partially discerned at any stage in the visual hierarchy, messages are sent back to earlier stages to alert them to certain locations or features in order to look for additional evidence for the object (and these processes may be facilitated by direct limbic activation). Finally, given constraints on allocation of attentional resources, art is most appealing if it produces heightened activity in a single dimension (e.g. through the peak shift principle or through grouping) rather than redundant activation of multiple modules. This idea may help explain the effectiveness of outline drawings and sketches, the savant syndrome in autists, and the sudden emergence of artistic talent in fronto-temporal dementia. In addition to these three basic principles we propose five others, constituting a total of ‘eight laws of aesthetic experience’ (analogous to the Buddha’s eightfold path to

[1]  R. Arnheim Art And Visual Perception , 1954 .

[2]  F. Attneave Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.

[3]  J. Kaas,et al.  Representation of the visual field in striate and adjoining cortex of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). , 1971, Brain research.

[4]  J. Pokorny Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1972 .

[5]  G. Johansson Visual motion perception. , 1975, Scientific American.

[6]  D H Hubel,et al.  Brain mechanisms of vision. , 1979, Scientific American.

[7]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Two‐dimensional maps of the cerebral cortex , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[8]  Sandy Lovie How the mind works , 1980, Nature.

[9]  S. Zeki The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex , 1980, Nature.

[10]  R. Bauer,et al.  Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  A. Damasio,et al.  Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. , 1985, Science.

[12]  H. Barlow Why have multiple cortical areas? , 1986, Vision Research.

[13]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  A. Damasio,et al.  Non-conscious face recognition in patients with face agnosia , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  Vilayanur S. Ramachandran,et al.  Theories of Perception. , 1951 .

[16]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A critique of pure vision , 1993 .

[17]  W Singer,et al.  Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  A. Snyder,et al.  Autistic Artists Give Clues to Cognition , 1997, Perception.

[19]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Capgras syndrome: a novel probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and familiarity of persons , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Object recognition can drive motion perception , 1998, Nature.

[21]  C. Koch,et al.  Consciousness and neuroscience. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Abi Berger,et al.  Phantoms in the brain , 1999, BMJ.

[23]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.