On the validity of the preception hypothesis.

The preception hypothesis holds that when an aversive stimulus is made predictable in time, S may be able to inhibit his arousal response to that stimulus and thus reduce its aversiveness or impact. In response to a paper by Furedy and Klajner, which questions the validity of the preception hypothesis, it is argued that considerable behavioral and autonomic response data seem to support this hypothesis. The present authors, in particular, do not share Furedy and Klajner's view that OR theory developed to explain habituation should also be adopted to account for the findings attributed to preception. On the contrary, preception is proposed to be a distinct process playing a role complementary to that of habituation, more labile and also more effortful in the sense that it requires the involvement of the “second signal system,” while habituation can apparently proceed as a non-cognitive, automatic process. However, Furedy and Klajner's discussion suggests the fruitfulness of additional investigations to test more decisively differential predictions based on an habituation vs a preception position. Even if accepted the preception mechanism could ultimately be incorporated in an elaborated version of OR theory.

[1]  W. Grings,et al.  Judgments of UCS intensity and diminution of the unconditioned GSR. , 1971, Psychophysiology.

[2]  W. Grings,et al.  Magnitude of electrodermal response to a standard stimulus as a function of intensity and proximity of a prior stimulus. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  M. Seligman Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  S Epstein,et al.  Expectancy and magnitude of reaction to a noxious UCS. , 1973, Psychophysiology.

[5]  J. Harsh,et al.  Choice of longer or stronger signalled shock over shorter or weaker unsignalled shock. , 1973, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[6]  W. Grings,et al.  Magnitude of UCR as a function of variability in the CS-UCS relationship. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  W. Roth,et al.  Auditory evoked responses to unpredictable stimuli. , 1973, Psychophysiology.

[8]  J. Furedy,et al.  On evaluating autonomic and verbal indices of negative preception. , 1974, Psychophysiology.

[9]  D. Lykken Preception in the Rat: Autonomic Response to Shock as Function of Length of Warning Interval , 1962, Science.

[10]  Classical aversive conditioning of human digital volume-pulse change and tests of the preparatory-adaptive-response interpretation of the reinforcement. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  C. I. Howarth,et al.  Changes in Threshold Level Produced by a Signal Preceding or following the Threshold Stimulus , 1959 .

[12]  J. Lanzetta,et al.  Preference for information about an uncertain but unavoidable outcome. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  J. Geer,et al.  Predictable and unpredictable aversive events: Evidence for the safety-signal hypothesis , 1972 .

[14]  A. Tellegen,et al.  Perception: autonomic response to shock as a function of predictability in time and locus. , 1972, Psychophysiology.

[15]  J. Furedy Test of the preparatory adaptive response interpretation of aversive classical autonomic conditioning. , 1970 .

[16]  J. Furedy,et al.  Failures of information to reduce rated aversiveness of unmodifiable shock , 1971 .

[17]  Unconfounded autonomic indexes of the aversiveness of signaled and unsignaled shocks. , 1972 .

[18]  J. Weiss,et al.  Somatic Effects of Predictable and Unpredictable Shock , 1970, Psychosomatic medicine.