The effects of video game use on performance in a virtual navigation task

Video games are a popular form of entertainment, but computerized tasks, often resembling video games or built off of video game platforms, are increasingly being used in research. Previous studies have indicated a correlation between video game use and performance on many virtual tasks, including tasks of navigation and orientation. By more completely characterizing previous video game experience and controlling for increased dexterity with game controls, this study endeavored to clarify the nature of such improvements in performance. The findings confirmed that individuals with a longer history of video game play have better performance on a virtual navigation task; however, this effect was greatest in players of video games with a navigational component, and it was not due solely to better use of game controls. Furthermore, participants who play navigational video games more often reported using more efficient navigational strategies, such as using cognitive maps or adopting procedural approaches through learned routes. The findings reported in this study provide evidence that better navigation and topographical orientation skills in individuals playing video games are likely due to the consistent practice of those skills while playing for the purpose of entertainment. Length of video game experience correlated with performance on navigation task.Improved performance was seen in those who play video games that include navigation.Improved motor control only partially explained improvement in task performance.People practiced in navigation video games used efficient navigation strategies.

[1]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Developmental topographical disorientation: Case one , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Action video games and informal education: Effects on strategies for dividing visual attention , 1994 .

[3]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[4]  C. Ferguson Do Angry Birds Make for Angry Children? A Meta-Analysis of Video Game Influences on Children’s and Adolescents’ Aggression, Mental Health, Prosocial Behavior, and Academic Performance , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[5]  J. Barrash A historical review of topographical disorientation and its neuroanatomical correlates. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[6]  Douglas Landsittel,et al.  Can video games be used to predict or improve laparoscopic skills? , 2005, Journal of endourology.

[7]  I. Ensari,et al.  Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates , 2013, International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity.

[8]  U. Polat,et al.  Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Video game experience predicts virtual, but not real navigation performance , 2011, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[10]  C S Green,et al.  Action-Video-Game Experience Alters the Spatial Resolution of Vision , 2007, Psychological science.

[11]  Jeffrey S. Taube,et al.  Is Navigation in Virtual Reality with fMRI Really Navigation? , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Kasey L. Powers,et al.  Effects of video-game play on information processing: A meta-analytic investigation , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[13]  S. Mitroff,et al.  Video game players show more precise multisensory temporal processing abilities , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Aysecan Boduroglu,et al.  Action video game players form more detailed representation of objects. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[15]  P N Wilson,et al.  Uses of virtual reality in clinical training: developing the spatial skills of children with mobility impairments. , 1998, Studies in health technology and informatics.

[16]  Leanne M. Casey,et al.  Outcomes associated with virtual reality in psychological interventions: where are we now? , 2014, Clinical psychology review.

[17]  Margaret D. Nolan,et al.  Vection and simulator sickness. , 1990, Military psychology : the official journal of the Division of Military Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[18]  Valeska Gatica-Rojas,et al.  Virtual reality interface devices in the reorganization of neural networks in the brain of patients with neurological diseases , 2014, Neural regeneration research.

[19]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[20]  S. Kühn,et al.  Amount of lifetime video gaming is positively associated with entorhinal, hippocampal and occipital volume , 2014, Molecular Psychiatry.

[21]  C. Geiser,et al.  The relationship between computer-game preference, gender, and mental-rotation ability , 2006 .

[22]  Giuliano Carlo Geminiani,et al.  Spatial navigation in large-scale virtual environments: Gender differences in survey tasks , 2008, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[23]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Quantitative and qualitative sex differences in spatial navigation. , 2007, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[24]  Nicole Etchamendy,et al.  Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town , 2007, Hippocampus.

[25]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  Cognitive mapping in humans and its relationship to other orientation skills , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  E. Spelke,et al.  Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .

[27]  C. S. Green,et al.  Children, Wired: For Better and for Worse , 2010, Neuron.

[28]  Aishah Bakhtiar,et al.  Growing up multitasking: The costs and benefits for cognitive development , 2015 .

[29]  Richard De Lisi,et al.  Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance , 1996 .

[30]  N. Burgess Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Julie I. Cohen,et al.  Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[33]  R. Mayer,et al.  Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. , 2002 .

[34]  Corey J. Bohil,et al.  Virtual reality in neuroscience research and therapy , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  E A Maguire,et al.  Hippocampal involvement in human topographical memory: evidence from functional imaging. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[36]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  Neural Network Configuration and Efficiency Underlies Individual Differences in Spatial Orientation Ability , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  G. Iaria,et al.  Mental imagery skills and topographical orientation in humans: A correlation study , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  Tim Wright,et al.  An investigation of the validity of the virtual spatial navigation assessment , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[39]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Multiple interacting brain areas underlie successful spatiotemporal memory retrieval in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[40]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  D. Simons,et al.  The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[42]  Pablo Gagliardo,et al.  Virtual reality and cognitive rehabilitation: a review of current outcome research. , 2014, NeuroRehabilitation.

[43]  J L Griffith,et al.  Differences in Eye-Hand Motor Coordination of Video-Game Users and Non-Users , 1983, Perceptual and motor skills.

[44]  R. de Lisi,et al.  Improving Children's Mental Rotation Accuracy With Computer Game Playing , 2002, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[45]  M. Pépin,et al.  Effect of Playing a Video Game on a Measure of Spatial Visualization , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[46]  Glenda Revelle,et al.  Applying developmental theory and research to the creation of educational games. , 2013, New directions for child and adolescent development.

[47]  Kathy A. Low,et al.  Performance gains from directed training do not transfer to untrained tasks. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[48]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  How Immersive Is Enough? A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Immersive Technology on User Presence , 2016 .

[49]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[50]  Renjie Li,et al.  Perceptual Learning During Action Video Game Playing , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[51]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Virtual Navigation Performance: The Relationship to Field of View and Prior Video Gaming Experience , 2011, Perceptual and motor skills.

[52]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. , 2002 .

[53]  M. Pokorski,et al.  Computer games and fine motor skills. , 2013, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[54]  P. Frensch,et al.  Effects of video game playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. , 1994 .

[55]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning , 2006 .

[56]  Fran C. Blumberg,et al.  Introduction: digital games as a context for cognitive development, learning, and developmental research. , 2013, New directions for child and adolescent development.

[57]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Developmental topographical disorientation: a newly discovered cognitive disorder , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  C. S. Green,et al.  Enumeration versus multiple object tracking: the case of action video game players , 2006, Cognition.