Proprioceptive versus visual control in autistic children

The autistic child's presumed preference for proximal rather than distal sensory input was studied by requiring that autistic, retarded, and normal subjects adapt to a prism-induced lateral displacement of the visual field. Only autistic subjects demonstrated transfer of adaptation to the nonadapted hand, indicative of a reliance on proprioception rather than vision to accomplish adaptation. Such reliance on proprioception was explained as an alternative strategy compensating for an inability to use current visual control of reaching rather than as a preference for proximal information per se.

[1]  U. Frith,et al.  The role of visual and motor cues for normal, subnormal and autistic children. , 1969, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[2]  I. Rock,et al.  Vision and touch. , 1967, Scientific American.

[3]  B. Freeman,et al.  National Society for Autistic Children definition of the syndrome of autism. , 1978, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[4]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Observations on the development of reaching for moving objects. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  W. Goldfarb Receptor preferences in schizophrenic children. , 1956, A.M.A. archives of neurology and psychiatry.

[6]  W. Pond,et al.  Modern Pork Production , 1983 .

[7]  W. Hays Statistics for psychologists , 1963 .

[8]  M M Cohen,et al.  Continuous versus Terminal Visual Feedback in Prism Aftereffects , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[9]  C. S. Harris Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.

[10]  N. O’connor,et al.  Psychological Experiments With Autistic Children , 1970 .

[11]  Sibylle K. Escalona,et al.  Unusual Sensitivities in Very Young Children , 1947 .

[12]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  An aetiological approach to the diagnosis of psychosis in childhood. , 1958 .

[14]  H H EVELOFF,et al.  The autistic child. , 1960, Archives of general psychiatry.

[15]  A. Kornheiser Adaptation to laterally displaced vision: a review. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  E. Schopler Visual versus tactual receptor preference in normal and schizophrenic children. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[17]  L. Wing,et al.  Early childhood autism : clinical, educational and social aspects , 1966 .

[18]  B. Hermelin Locating events in space and time: Experiments with autistic, blind, and deaf children , 1972, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[19]  E. Schopler Early infantile autism and receptor processes. , 1965, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Development in Infancy , 1982 .

[21]  P. Mcdonnell,et al.  The development of visually guided reaching , 1975 .

[22]  Van Krevelen Da Early infantile autism , 1952 .

[23]  J. Bruner Organization of early skilled action. , 1973, Child development.

[24]  E. Ornitz,et al.  Neurophysiologic mechanisms underlying perceptual inconstancy in autistic and schizophrenic children. , 1968, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. , 1933 .

[26]  E. Ornitz,et al.  The modulation of sensory input and motor output in autistic children , 1974, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[27]  Van Krevelend ON THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN EARLY INFANTILE AUTISM AND AUTISTIC PSYCHOPATHY. , 1963, Acta paedopsychiatrica.

[28]  E. Ornitz,et al.  Perceptual inconstancy in early infantile autism. The syndrome of early infant autism and its variants including certain cases of childhood schizophrenia. , 1968, Archives of general psychiatry.

[29]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .