Neural systems supporting timing and chronometric counting: an FMRI study.

At least two strategies are available to humans for estimating multisecond intervals. One depends on an interval timing system that is common to many species. The other is the language-based strategy of chronometric counting. These two strategies are easily distinguished by the psychophysical properties of their behavioral correlates: counting supports substantially more precise estimates than are possible using the more general interval timing system. The present study investigates the neural systems that underlie the execution of these different strategies. Eighteen adults reproduced a 16-s interval either by internally timing or covertly counting the duration. Comparison of counting and timing to a resting baseline suggested that these strategies engage some nonoverlapping neural systems. Counting, but not timing, strongly activated Broca's area, primary motor cortex in the mouth region, and right cerebellum, all of which are associated with internal speech. Counting also activated parts of the medial premotor circuit, including the putamen, supplementary motor area (SMA) proper, and cingulate motor area (CMA), that have been associated with reproducing isochronous and syncopated rhythms of elements lasting hundreds of milliseconds. During timing, only a portion of this circuit, the SMA proper and CMA, was engaged. Both timing and counting interfered with semantic processing during the resting state, evidenced by task-related decreases in the left inferior and middle frontal gyri, right superior frontal gyrus, left angular gyrus, and bilateral posterior cingulate cortex. This study suggests that counting activates a corticostriatal network associated with millisecond, rhythmic timing. In contrast, timing long durations without the benefit of linguistic strategies for subdividing counts reduces activity in this circuitry.

[1]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[2]  B. Gulyás,et al.  Cortical representation of self‐paced finger movement , 1996, Neuroreport.

[3]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Dissociation of Storage and Rehearsal in Verbal Working Memory: Evidence From Positron Emission Tomography , 1996 .

[4]  L. E. Talton,et al.  Interval schedule performance in the goldfish Carassius auratus , 1999, Behavioural Processes.

[5]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Evidence Linking Visual Object Enumeration and Attention , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Motor Timing: A PET Study of Auditory and Visual Rhythm Reproduction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[9]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Temporal integration in duration and number discrimination. , 1985 .

[10]  R Turner,et al.  Cortical and subcortical control of tongue movement in humans: a functional neuroimaging study using fMRI. , 1999, Journal of applied physiology.

[11]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[12]  David C. Van Essen,et al.  Application of Information Technology: An Integrated Software Suite for Surface-based Analyses of Cerebral Cortex , 2001, J. Am. Medical Informatics Assoc..

[13]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[14]  W. Meck,et al.  Increasing the speed of an internal clock: The effects of nicotine on interval timing , 1996 .

[15]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Erratum: "One-thousand one ... one-thousand two ...": Chronometric counting violates the scalar property in interval timing (Psychonomic Bulletin and Review (2002) 11, 1 (24-30)) , 2004 .

[18]  J. Desmond,et al.  Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[20]  D. Harrington,et al.  Neural Underpinnings of Temporal Processing: Α Review of Focal Lesion, Pharmacological, and Functional Imaging Research , 1999, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[21]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Programming the duration of a motor sequence: role of the primary and supplementary motor areas in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[24]  Mingxiong Huang,et al.  Neural representation of interval encoding and decision making. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  C Büchel,et al.  Brain regions involved in articulation , 1999, The Lancet.

[27]  S. Roberts,et al.  Isolation of an internal clock. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  R W Cox,et al.  Event‐related fMRI of tasks involving brief motion , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[29]  E. T. Possing,et al.  Neural Correlates of Lexical Access during Visual Word Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Church,et al.  The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[31]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  PET Evidence for an Amodal Verbal Working Memory System , 1996, NeuroImage.

[33]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Three-Dimensional MRI Atlas of the Human Cerebellum in Proportional Stereotaxic Space , 1999, NeuroImage.

[34]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Encyclopedia of the Human Brain , 2002 .

[35]  Simon Grondin,et al.  When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process of humans. , 1999 .

[36]  M. Hepp-Reymond,et al.  Reproducibility of primary motor cortex somatotopy under controlled conditions. , 2002, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[37]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  “One-thousandone … one-thousandtwo …”: Chronometric counting violates the scalar property in interval timing , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  M. Molinari,et al.  Verbal short-term store-rehearsal system and the cerebellum. Evidence from a patient with a right cerebellar lesion. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Human cerebellum plays an important role in memory-timed finger movement: an fMRI study. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  F. Binkofski,et al.  Cerebral correlates of working memory for temporal information , 2000, NeuroReport.

[41]  Deborah L. Harrington,et al.  Time Passage, Neural Substrates , 2002 .

[42]  A. R. Gilliland,et al.  Some factors in estimating short time intervals , 1940 .

[43]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[44]  J. Tanji,et al.  The role of premotor cortex and the supplementary motor area in the temporal control of movement in man. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  N Burgess,et al.  Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal short-term memory: an fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[47]  B A Schneider,et al.  A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[48]  S. Hinton Neuroimaging approaches to the study of interval timing. , 2003 .

[49]  V Haughton,et al.  Single- and multiple-event paradigms for identification of motor cortex activation. , 2000, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[50]  M Lotze,et al.  The representation of articulation in the primary sensorimotor cortex , 2000, Neuroreport.

[51]  W. Meck Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .

[52]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Franck Vidal,et al.  The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  E E Smith,et al.  Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  A componential analysis of pacemaker-counter timing systems. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  F. Vidal,et al.  Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  I. Peñuelas,et al.  Sustained Attention in a Counting Task: Normal Performance and Functional Neuroanatomy , 2001, NeuroImage.

[59]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[60]  K Ugurbil,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of Broca's area during internal speech. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[61]  John H. Wearden,et al.  Interval production as an analogue of the peak procedure: Evidence for similarity of human and animal timing processes , 1988 .

[62]  Jun Tanji,et al.  New concepts of the supplementary motor area , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[63]  N D Lerner,et al.  Duration discrimination by rats. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[64]  D. Mumford,et al.  Neural activity in early visual cortex reflects behavioral experience and higher-order perceptual saliency , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  W. N. Schoenfeld The Theory of reinforcement schedules , 1970 .

[67]  P. T. Fox,et al.  Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing , 1988, Nature.

[68]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[69]  John H. Wearden,et al.  Do humans possess an internal clock with scalar timing properties , 1991 .