Emotional Biases and Recurrence in Major Depressive Disorder. Results of 2.5 Years Follow-Up of Drug-Free Cohort Vulnerable for Recurrence

An interesting factor explaining recurrence risk in Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) may be neuropsychological functioning, i.e., processing of emotional stimuli/information. Negatively biased processing of emotional stimuli/information has been found in both acute and (inconclusively) remitted states of MDD, and may be causally related to recurrence of depression. We aimed to investigate self-referent, memory and interpretation biases in recurrently depressed patients in remission and relate these biases to recurrence. We included 69 remitted recurrent MDD-patients (rrMDD-patients), 35–65 years, with ≥2 episodes, voluntarily free of antidepressant maintenance therapy for at least 4 weeks. We tested self-referent biases with an emotional categorization task, bias in emotional memory by free recall of the emotion categorization task 15 min after completing it, and interpretation bias with a facial expression recognition task. We compared these participants with 43 never-depressed controls matched for age, sex and intelligence. We followed the rrMDD-patients for 2.5 years and assessed recurrent depressive episodes by structured interview. The rrMDD-patients showed biases toward emotionally negative stimuli, faster responses to negative self-relevant characteristics in the emotional categorization, better recognition of sad faces, worse recognition of neutral faces with more misclassifications as angry or disgusting faces and less misclassifications as neutral faces (0.001 < p < 0.05). Of these, the number of misclassifications as angry and the overall performance in the emotional memory task were significantly associated with the time to recurrence (p ≤ 0.04), independent of residual symptoms and number of previous episodes. In a support vector machine data-driven model, prediction of recurrence-status could best be achieved (relative to observed recurrence-rate) with demographic and childhood adversity parameters (accuracy 78.1%; 1-sided p = 0.002); neuropsychological tests could not improve this prediction. Our data suggests a persisting (mood-incongruent) emotional bias when patients with recurrent depression are in remission. Moreover, these persisting biases might be mechanistically important for recurrence and prevention thereof.

[1]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Depression: A cognitive perspective. , 2019, Clinical psychology review.

[2]  Katja Plemenitas,et al.  Cognitive Perspective , 2019, Decision-Making in American Foreign Policy.

[3]  G. Dawson,et al.  Predicting treatment response to antidepressant medication using early changes in emotional processing , 2019, European Neuropsychopharmacology.

[4]  I. Tendolkar,et al.  Cognitive bias modification as an add-on treatment in clinical depression: Results from a placebo-controlled, single-blinded randomized control trial. , 2018, Journal of affective disorders.

[5]  A. Schene,et al.  The measurement of cognitive reactivity to sad mood in patients remitted from major depressive disorder , 2018, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[6]  Louise Sharpe,et al.  Cognitive bias modification: A review of meta-analyses. , 2017, Journal of affective disorders.

[7]  Ioana R. Podina,et al.  A comprehensive meta-analysis of interpretation biases in depression. , 2017, Clinical psychology review.

[8]  A. Schene,et al.  Aberrant default-mode network-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-depression , 2017, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[9]  J. Moll,et al.  Early life stress explains reduced positive memory biases in remitted depression , 2017, European Psychiatry.

[10]  A. Aleman,et al.  Associations Between Daily Affective Instability and Connectomics in Functional Subnetworks in Remitted Patients with Recurrent Major Depressive Disorder , 2017, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[11]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Negative Self-Referential Processing Predicts the Recurrence of Major Depressive Episodes , 2017, Clinical psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[12]  E. Koster,et al.  Attentional bias temporal dynamics in remitted depression. , 2016, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  C. Harmer,et al.  Vulnerability for new episodes in recurrent major depressive disorder: protocol for the longitudinal DELTA-neuroimaging cohort study , 2016, BMJ Open.

[14]  A. Young,et al.  The role of self-blame and worthlessness in the psychopathology of major depressive disorder , 2015, Journal of affective disorders.

[15]  B. Franke,et al.  Interaction of the 5-HTTLPR and childhood trauma influences memory bias in healthy individuals. , 2015, Journal of affective disorders.

[16]  E. Koster,et al.  Cognitive bias modification for depression , 2015 .

[17]  N. Farb,et al.  A two-factor model of relapse/recurrence vulnerability in unipolar depression. , 2015, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  Kerri Smith,et al.  Mental health: A world of depression , 2014, Nature.

[19]  A. Arias-Vasquez,et al.  What is the contribution of different cognitive biases and stressful childhood events to the presence and number of previous depressive episodes? , 2014, Psychiatry Research.

[20]  B. Franke,et al.  Association between genes, stressful childhood events and processing bias in depression vulnerable individuals , 2014, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[21]  M. Phillips,et al.  Neural correlates of dysfunctional emotion regulation in major depressive disorder. A systematic review of neuroimaging studies , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  P. Putman,et al.  A Multiple Case Series Analysis of Six Variants of Attentional Bias Modification for Depression , 2013, ISRN psychiatry.

[23]  P. Cowen,et al.  Effects of Short-Term Varenicline Administration on Emotional and Cognitive Processing in Healthy, Non-Smoking Adults: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Study , 2013, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[24]  B. Franke,et al.  Never-depressed females with a family history of depression demonstrate affective bias , 2013, Psychiatry Research.

[25]  R. Atchley,et al.  Depression reduces perceptual sensitivity for positive words and pictures , 2012, Cognition & emotion.

[26]  E. Holmes,et al.  Using Attentional Bias Modification as a Cognitive Vaccine Against Depression , 2012, Biological Psychiatry.

[27]  Mary L. Phillips,et al.  Bias to negative emotions: A depression state-dependent marker in adolescent major depressive disorder , 2012, Psychiatry Research.

[28]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognitive Aspects of Depression. , 2012, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science.

[29]  Andrew Mathews,et al.  Cognitive Bias Modification , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[30]  C. Sears,et al.  Attention to Emotional Images in Previously Depressed Individuals: An Eye-Tracking Study , 2011, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[31]  P. Cowen,et al.  A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action , 2011, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[32]  Sophie Guionnet,et al.  Brain effects of antidepressants in major depression: a meta-analysis of emotional processing studies. , 2011, Journal of affective disorders.

[33]  J. Joormann,et al.  Components of interference control predict depressive symptoms and rumination cross-sectionally and at six months follow-up. , 2011, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[34]  M. Otto,et al.  A meta‐analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression , 2010, Depression and anxiety.

[35]  Monique Ernst,et al.  Attention Bias Modification Treatment: A Meta-Analysis Toward the Establishment of Novel Treatment for Anxiety , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[36]  A. Schene,et al.  Prediction of recurrence in recurrent depression: a 5.5-year prospective study. , 2010, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[37]  J. van os,et al.  Unveiling patterns of affective responses in daily life may improve outcome prediction in depression: a momentary assessment study. , 2010, Journal of affective disorders.

[38]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. , 2010, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[39]  Ernst H. W. Koster,et al.  Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[40]  J. Joormann,et al.  Attentional control in depression: A translational affective neuroscience approach , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  C. Harmer Antidepressant drug action: a neuropsychological perspective , 2010, Depression and anxiety.

[42]  Donatella Marazziti,et al.  Cognitive impairment in major depression. , 2010, European journal of pharmacology.

[43]  A. Schene,et al.  Long-term effects of preventive cognitive therapy in recurrent depression: a 5.5-year follow-up study. , 2009, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[44]  R. de Graaf,et al.  Prevalence and predictors of recurrence of major depressive disorder in the adult population , 2009, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[45]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Identification of emotional facial expressions following recovery from depression. , 2009, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[46]  P. Cowen,et al.  Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. , 2009, The American journal of psychiatry.

[47]  A. von Leupoldt,et al.  Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients with asthma , 2009, Psychological Medicine.

[48]  P. Cowen,et al.  Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action , 2009, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[49]  R. Raedt,et al.  Impairments in cognitive control persist during remission from depression and are related to the number of past episodes: An event related potentials study , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[50]  D. Le Gall,et al.  Cognitive inhibition and working memory in unipolar depression. , 2009, Journal of affective disorders.

[51]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Rethinking Rumination , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[52]  N. Allen,et al.  The Impact of Intensive Mindfulness Training on Attentional Control, Cognitive Style, and Affect , 2008, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[53]  Chongde Lin,et al.  The neural mechanism underlying the female advantage in identifying negative emotions: An event-related potential study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[54]  Alishia D. Williams,et al.  An investigation of the relationship between cognitive reactivity and rumination. , 2008, Behavior therapy.

[55]  F. Schmidt Meta-Analysis , 2008 .

[56]  D. Wentura,et al.  Dysphorics cannot ignore unpleasant information , 2007 .

[57]  Stella W. Y. Chan,et al.  Highly neurotic never-depressed students have negative biases in information processing , 2007, Psychological Medicine.

[58]  Sheri L. Johnson,et al.  Does processing of emotional stimuli predict symptomatic improvement and diagnostic recovery from major depression? , 2007, Emotion.

[59]  Lisa Talbot,et al.  Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. , 2007, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[60]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. , 2007, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[61]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. , 2006, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[62]  L. Leyman,et al.  Attentional biases for angry faces in unipolar depression , 2006, Psychological Medicine.

[63]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. , 2006, Journal of affective disorders.

[64]  Z. Segal,et al.  Cognitive reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse. , 2006, Archives of general psychiatry.

[65]  A. Schene,et al.  Differential Predictors of Response to Preventive Cognitive Therapy in Recurrent Depression: A 2-Year Prospective Study , 2006, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics.

[66]  J. Leppänen Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings , 2006, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[67]  Yi-Min Huang,et al.  Weighted support vector machine for classification with uneven training class sizes , 2005, 2005 International Conference on Machine Learning and Cybernetics.

[68]  Z. Segal,et al.  Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. , 2005, Clinical psychology review.

[69]  J. Hietanen,et al.  Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces , 2004, Psychiatry Research.

[70]  V. Dodin,et al.  Emotional information processing in first and recurrent major depressive episodes. , 2004, Journal of psychiatric research.

[71]  P. Andersen,et al.  The predictive effect of episodes on the risk of recurrence in depressive and bipolar disorders – a life‐long perspective , 2004, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[72]  A. Young,et al.  Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[73]  P. Hertel Memory for Emotional and Nonemotional Events in Depression: A Question of Habit? , 2004 .

[74]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes , 2004 .

[75]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. , 2002, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[76]  M. Stein,et al.  The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in a Community Sample: Psychometric Properties and Normative Data , 2001, Journal of traumatic stress.

[77]  G. Goodwin,et al.  Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology , 2001, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[78]  R. Ingram,et al.  Cognitive reactivity and depressotypic information processing in children of depressed mothers. , 1999, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[79]  A. Bouhuys,et al.  Gender-specific mechanisms associated with outcome of depression: perception of emotions, coping and interpersonal functioning , 1999, Psychiatry Research.

[80]  R G Robinson,et al.  Cognitive impairment in the euthymic phase of chronic unipolar depression. , 1997, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[81]  J Foote,et al.  Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. , 1994, The American journal of psychiatry.

[82]  D. Kupfer,et al.  The definition and operational criteria for treatment outcome of major depressive disorder. A review of the current research literature. , 1991, Archives of general psychiatry.

[83]  B Schmand,et al.  [The Dutch Reading Test for Adults: a measure of premorbid intelligence level]. , 1991, Tijdschrift voor gerontologie en geriatrie.

[84]  K. Dobson,et al.  Specificity and stability of self-referent encoding in clinical depression. , 1987, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[85]  C. Hammen,et al.  Self-schemas, depression, and the processing of personal information in children. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[86]  N. Kuiper,et al.  Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. , 1981, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[87]  M. Seligman,et al.  Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. , 1978, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[88]  M. Hamilton,et al.  Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. , 1967, The British journal of social and clinical psychology.

[89]  F. Verhage,et al.  [AN ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE BASED ON THE GRONINGER INTELLIGENCE TEST SCORES]. , 1964, Nederlands tijdschrift voor de psychologie en haar grensgebieden.

[90]  A. Beekman,et al.  A 2-year prospective study , 2017 .

[91]  P. Cuijpers,et al.  Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis. , 2015, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[92]  Stuart Moss,et al.  Current Status and Future Directions , 2013 .

[93]  L. Buliński,et al.  A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE , 2015 .

[94]  Trevor Hastie,et al.  Model Assessment and Selection , 2009 .

[95]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. , 2005, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[96]  T. Joiner,et al.  Depression in its interpersonal context. , 2002 .

[97]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. , 1994, Annual review of psychology.

[98]  B. Ineichen,et al.  World depression. , 1992, Nursing the elderly : in hospital, homes and the community.