Visual perception of motion and 3-D structure from motion: an fMRI study.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to study the cortical bases of 3-D structure perception from visual motion in human. Nine subjects underwent three experiments designed to locate the areas involved in (i) motion processing (random motion versus static dots), (ii) coherent motion processing (expansion/ contraction versus random motion) and (iii) 3-D shape from motion reconstruction (3-D surface oscillating in depth versus random motion). Two control experiments tested the specific influence of speed distribution and surface curvature on the activation results. All stimuli consisted of random dots so that motion parallax was the only cue available for 3-D shape perception. As expected, random motion compared with static dots induced strong activity in areas V1/V2, V5+ and the superior occipital gyrus (SOG; presumptive V3/V3A). V1/V2 and V5+ showed no activity increase when comparing coherent motion (expansion or 3-D surface) with random motion. Conversely, V3/V3A and the dorsal parieto-occipital junction were highlighted in both comparisons and showed gradually increased activity for random motion, coherent motion and a curved surface rotating in depth, which suggests their involvement in the coding of 3-D shape from motion. Also, the ventral aspect of the left occipito-temporal junction was found to be equally responsive to random and coherent motion stimuli, but showed a specific sensitivity to curved 3-D surfaces compared with plane surfaces. As this region is already known to be involved in the coding of static object shape, our results suggest that it might integrate various cues for the perception of 3-D shape.

[1]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Inferior parietal lobule. Divergent architectonic asymmetries in the human brain. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[2]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[3]  S Zeki,et al.  Going beyond the information given: the relation of illusory visual motion to brain activity , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  B. Rogers,et al.  Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception , 1982, Vision Research.

[6]  P. Jezzard,et al.  Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[7]  G J Andersen,et al.  Shape and depth perception from parallel projections of three-dimensional motion. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  The Representation of Movement in Near Extra-Personal Space in the Macaque Ventral Intraparietal Area (VIP) , 1997 .

[9]  John C. Gore,et al.  Brain activation associated with visual motion studied by functional magnetic resonance imaging in humans , 1994 .

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Orban,et al.  Selectivity of Macaque MT/V5 Neurons for Surface Orientation in Depth Specified by Motion , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  J F Norman,et al.  The detection of surface curvatures defined by optical motion , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  A. Treisman,et al.  Parietal contributions to visual feature binding: evidence from a patient with bilateral lesions , 1995, Science.

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  G. Orban,et al.  Many areas in the human brain respond to visual motion. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  S. Zeki,et al.  The cerebral activity related to the visual perception of forward motion in depth. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[18]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Functional specialization for visual motion processing in primate cerebral cortex. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[19]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual motion coherence , 1992, Nature.

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction , 1995, Science.

[21]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[23]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[24]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  R. Malach,et al.  Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  K Cheng,et al.  Human cortical regions activated by wide-field visual motion: an H2(15)O PET study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Disparity sensitivity of neurons in monkey extrastriate area MST , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Regan,et al.  Binocular and monocular stimuli for motion in depth: Changing-disparity and changing-size feed the same motion-in-depth stage , 1979, Vision Research.

[31]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  R. Andersen,et al.  Encoding of three-dimensional structure-from-motion by primate area MT neurons , 1998, Nature.

[33]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  A. Treisman,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[37]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[38]  A. Sehgal,et al.  Positional Cloning and Sequence Analysis of the Drosophila Clock Gene, timeless , 1995, Science.

[39]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Graded effects of spatial and featural attention on human area MT and associated motion processing areas. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  A. Cowey,et al.  Brain damage and global stereopsis , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[41]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Paul Chauchard,et al.  Le cerveau humain , 1963 .

[43]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[44]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  J. Droulez,et al.  The visual perception of three-dimensional shape from self-motion and object-motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[46]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Comparison of neuronal selectivity for stimulus speed, length, and contrast in the prestriate visual cortical areas V4 and MT of the macaque monkey. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  R. S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The Activity in Human Areas V1/V2, V3, and V5 during the Perception of Coherent and Incoherent Motion , 1996, NeuroImage.

[48]  Andrea J. van Doorn,et al.  Invariant Properties of the Motion Parallax Field due to the Movement of Rigid Bodies Relative to an Observer , 1975 .

[49]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[50]  A. Verri,et al.  First-order analysis of optical flow in monkey brain. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  The effects of V4 and middle temporal (MT) area lesions on visual performance in the rhesus monkey , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Analysis of V3A and Related Areas in Human Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Andersen,et al.  Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques , 1995, Nature.

[54]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Structural and Functional Analyses of Human Cerebral Cortex Using a Surface-Based Atlas , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[57]  M. Taussig The Nervous System , 1991 .

[58]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Stereo-motion cooperation and the use ofmotion disparity in the visual perception of 3-D structure , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[59]  S. Ullman The interpretation of structure from motion , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[60]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information , 1995, Nature.