Children's learning and transfer of inductive reasoning rules: studies of proximal development.

An initial study examined the relation between current developmental levels, as estimated by IQ, and proximal levels of development, as estimated by the efficiency of learning and transfer in assisted contexts. 8-11-year-old children learned to solve letter series completion problems with the aid of graduated sequences of prompts. Maintenance and transfer were later assessed using similar prompting procedures. Both IQ and age effects were found. Average-IQ (and younger) children required more assistance than high-IQ (and older) children to achieve the same mastery level on the original problem types. In addition to this difference in learning efficiency, average-versus high-IQ students, and younger versus older children, subsequently diverged in the number of prompts they needed as the transfer problems differed increasingly from the ones originally learned. In a second study, amount of assistance required in learning was found to be quite stable across moderately related tasks (letter series and progressive matrices). Assistance needed in maintenance and transfer appeared less stable but was also significantly correlated across tasks. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed.

[1]  J. Carlson,et al.  Toward a differential testing approach: Testing-the-limits employing the Raven matrices☆ , 1979 .

[2]  Edward L. Thorndike,et al.  Intelligence and its measurement: A symposium--I. , 1921 .

[3]  Yaacov Rand,et al.  The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The learning potential assessment device, theory, instruments and techniques , 1981 .

[4]  R. Siegler Children's Thinking : What Develops? , 1978 .

[5]  Robert Glaser,et al.  Problem Solving and Intelligence. , 1975 .

[6]  R. Sternberg,et al.  What is Intelligence?: Contemporary Viewpoints on its Nature and Definition , 1986 .

[7]  H A SIMON,et al.  HUMAN ACQUISITION OF CONCEPTS FOR SEQUENTIAL PATTERNS. , 1963, Psychological review.

[8]  H. Simon,et al.  Empirical tests of a theory of human acquisition of concepts for sequential patterns , 1973 .

[9]  M. Chi Knowledge structures and memory development. , 1978 .

[10]  Walter F. Dearborn Intelligence and its measurement: A symposium--XII. , 1921 .

[11]  R. Sperber,et al.  Learning and cognition in the mentally retarded , 1984 .

[12]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Advances in developmental psychology , 1981 .

[13]  Frank C. Neil Intelligence and the rest of cognition , 1982 .

[14]  Paul I. Jacobs,et al.  Evaluating the Teaching of Intelligence1 , 1972 .

[15]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[16]  B. Fellows Chance stimulus sequences for discrimination tasks. , 1967, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  James V. Wertsch,et al.  Culture, communication, and cognition : Vygotskian perspectives , 1987 .

[18]  E. Thorndike The measurement of intelligence , 1924 .

[19]  B. Bloom Human Characteristics and School Learning , 1979 .

[20]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Breakdowns in flexible use of information: Intelligence-related differences in transfer following equivalent learning performance , 1985 .

[21]  Ann L. Brown Learning and Development: the Problems of Compatibility, Access and Induction. , 1982 .

[22]  Herbert Woodrow Intelligence and its measurement: A symposium--XI. , 1921 .

[23]  H. Woodrow The ability to learn. , 1946, Psychological review.

[24]  R. Sternberg,et al.  People's conceptions of intelligence. , 1981 .

[25]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  The zone of proximal development: Implications for individual differences and learning , 1984 .

[26]  Lev Vygotsky Mind in society , 1978 .

[27]  L. Vygotsky Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press , 1978 .

[28]  Use of testing-the-limits procedures in the assessment of intellectual capabilities in children with learning difficulties. , 1978, American journal of mental deficiency.