Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion

Typically developing human infants preferentially attend to biological motion within the first days of life. This ability is highly conserved across species and is believed to be critical for filial attachment and for detection of predators. The neural underpinnings of biological motion perception are overlapping with brain regions involved in perception of basic social signals such as facial expression and gaze direction, and preferential attention to biological motion is seen as a precursor to the capacity for attributing intentions to others. However, in a serendipitous observation, we recently found that an infant with autism failed to recognize point-light displays of biological motion, but was instead highly sensitive to the presence of a non-social, physical contingency that occurred within the stimuli by chance. This observation raised the possibility that perception of biological motion may be altered in children with autism from a very early age, with cascading consequences for both social development and the lifelong impairments in social interaction that are a hallmark of autism spectrum disorders. Here we show that two-year-olds with autism fail to orient towards point-light displays of biological motion, and their viewing behaviour when watching these point-light displays can be explained instead as a response to non-social, physical contingencies—physical contingencies that are disregarded by control children. This observation has far-reaching implications for understanding the altered neurodevelopmental trajectory of brain specialization in autism.

[1]  M. Uschold,et al.  Methods and applications , 1953 .

[2]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[3]  Ronald W. Schafer,et al.  Digital Processing of Speech Signals , 1978 .

[4]  M.G. Bellanger,et al.  Digital processing of speech signals , 1980, Proceedings of the IEEE.

[5]  R. Fox,et al.  The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.

[6]  R. Leigh,et al.  The neurology of eye movements , 1984 .

[7]  F. Greenberg,et al.  Williams syndrome. , 1988, Pediatrics.

[8]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 30 , 1995 .

[9]  R. Blake Cats Perceive Biological Motion , 1993 .

[10]  D. Perrett,et al.  Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  David C. Burr,et al.  Seeing biological motion , 1998, Nature.

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[13]  Nick Fogt,et al.  The Neurology of Eye Movements, 3rd ed. , 2000 .

[14]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence , 2000, Current Biology.

[15]  J. Harbison,et al.  The Neurology of Eye Movements, 3rd ed , 2000 .

[16]  M. Pavlova,et al.  Orientation specificity in biological motion perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Visual perception of biological motion in newly hatched chicks as revealed by an imprinting procedure , 2000, Animal Cognition.

[18]  Mark H. Johnson Functional brain development in humans , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  Birger Kollmeier,et al.  Estimation of the signal-to-noise ratio with amplitude modulation spectrograms , 2002, Speech Commun..

[21]  Ludmila I. Kuncheva Binomial distribution handbook for scientists and engineers , by Elart von Collani and Klaus Dräger. Pp.376. $79.95. 2001. ISBN 0 8176 4129 7 (Birkhäuser). , 2002 .

[22]  J. Hoffman,et al.  Intact Perception of Biological Motion in the Face of Profound Spatial Deficits: Williams Syndrome , 2002, Psychological science.

[23]  R. Scheuplein,et al.  Biological Basis , 2002 .

[24]  Peter Y. Chen,et al.  Correlation: Parametric and Nonparametric Measures , 2002 .

[25]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Visual Recognition of Biological Motion is Impaired in Children With Autism , 2003, Psychological science.

[26]  F. Volkmar,et al.  The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  R. Tuchman,et al.  [Autism and pervasive developmental disorders]. , 2004, Jornal de pediatria.

[28]  N. Troje,et al.  Differential involvement of the cerebellum in biological and coherent motion perception , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Visually Inexperienced Chicks Exhibit Spontaneous Preference for Biological Motion Patterns , 2005, PLoS biology.

[30]  T. Allison,et al.  Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Kim M. Dalton,et al.  Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Aina Puce,et al.  Configural Processing of Biological Motion in Human Superior Temporal Sulcus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Pierre Fonlupt,et al.  Listening to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area , 2005, NeuroImage.

[34]  R. Schultz Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[35]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Biological Motion: A Perceptual Life Detector? , 2006, Current Biology.

[36]  Gérard Chollet,et al.  MEASURING AUDIO AND VISUAL SPEECH SYNCHRONY: METHODS AND APPLICATIONS , 2006 .

[37]  Stefanie Hoehl,et al.  The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  A. Klin,et al.  Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Archives of general psychiatry.

[39]  Warren Jones,et al.  Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. , 2008, Developmental science.

[40]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Rapid naming deficits in dyslexia: a stumbling block for the perceptual anchor theory of dyslexia. , 2008, Developmental science.

[41]  F. Simion,et al.  A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Carolyn R. Bertozzi,et al.  Methods and Applications , 2009 .

[43]  Nasreen Razack-Malik Autism Spectrum Disorders in Infants and Toddlers: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment , 2009 .