Territorial defense and advertisement by footdrumming in bannertail kangaroo rats (Dipodomys spectabilis) at high and low population densities

SummaryBehaviors associated with territorial defense of large dirt mounds by bannertail kangaroo rats (Dipodomys spectabilis) at high and low population densities are reported. Rats were observed for 640 h during three summers and one spring from 1980–1982 in SE Arizona.Bannertail kangaroo rats defended their territories by footdrumming, as a long-distance warning signal, and chasing as a closer-distance threat. They footdrummed on or near their mounds spontaneosly during the night, in response to neighbors' footdrums, and durint mound challenges. There were no sexual differences in footdrumming, and juveniles frequently performed the behavior. The low frequency sounds were within the auditory sensitivities of the rats and footdrumming patterns differed somewhat between individuals. A playback experiment provided preliminary evidence that rats can differentiate between the footdrums of neighbors and strangers. When a bannertail visited the mound of another bannertail the mound owner actively defended its mound and immediately chased the visitor away. Fighting and mound challenges occurred infrequently, and the uniformly dispersed distribution of mounds suggested an effort by rats to avoid mutual interference.When population densities were high, dispersing juveniles built new mounds, consequently, distances between mounds decreased. Rats responded by tolerating closer neighbors, spending more time active on their mounds, and visiting neighboring mounds less. Adults increased footdrumming rates at high population densities, but juveniles footdrummed at high rates regardless of the population size.

[1]  PRE-COPULATORY AND COPULATORY BEHAVIOUR IN RELATION TO STAGES OF THE OESTROUS CYCLE IN THE FEMALE MONGOLIAN GERBIL , 1980 .

[2]  G. Orians,et al.  Spacing Patterns in Mobile Animals , 1970 .

[3]  S. Holman,et al.  Pre-copulatory behaviour in the male Mongolian gerbil: I. Differences in dependence on androgen of component patterns , 1982, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  C. Carter,et al.  Ovarian hormones, copulatory stimuli, and female sexual behavior in the Mongolian gerbil , 1980, Hormones and Behavior.

[5]  L. Beletsky An investigation of individual recognition by voice in female red-winged blackbirds , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  G. Schroder Foraging Behavior and Home Range Utilization of the Bannertial Kangaroo Rat (Dipodomys Spectabilis) , 1979 .

[7]  Christopher C. Smith The Adaptive Nature of Social Organization in the Genus of Three Squirrels Tamiasciurus , 1968 .

[8]  J. Fischer,et al.  Long-term estrogen and progesterone and mating stimuli as regulators of female sexual receptivity in the Mongolian gerbil. , 1980, Behavioral and neural biology.

[9]  G. Parker,et al.  Role assessment, reserve strategy, and acquisition of information in asymmetric animal conflicts , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  T. Best Mound Development by a Pioneer Population of the Banner-Tailed Kangaroo Rat, Dipodomys spectabilis baileyi Goldman, in Eastern New Mexico , 1972 .

[11]  B. Galef,et al.  A Sensitive Period for the Maintenance of Emotionality in Mongolian Gerbils , 1979 .

[12]  Robert J. Howe Agonistic Behavior of Three Sympatric Species of Woodrats (Neotoma mexicana, N. albigula, and N. stephensi) , 1978 .

[13]  D. Webster A Function of the Enlarged Middle-Ear Cavities of the Kangaroo Rat, Dipodomys , 1962, Physiological Zoology.

[14]  B. Galef,et al.  The role of the physical rearing environment in the domestication of the mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) , 1977, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  G. Monson Food Habits of the Banner-Tailed Kangaroo Rat in Arizona , 1943 .

[16]  Wayne Kessler,et al.  Life History Notes on the Banner-Tailed Kangaroo Rat, Merriam's Kangaroo Rat, and the White-Throated Wood Rat in Arizona and New Mexico , 1940 .

[17]  L. Elliott Social behavior and foraging ecology of the eastern chipmunk (Tamias striatus) in the Adirondack Mountains , 1978 .

[18]  Devra G. Kleiman,et al.  Advances in the study of mammalian behavior , 1983 .

[19]  Sokal Rr,et al.  Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research 2nd edition. , 1981 .

[20]  P. J. Clark,et al.  Distance to Nearest Neighbor as a Measure of Spatial Relationships in Populations , 1954 .

[21]  D. H. Owings,et al.  Snake‐directed Behavior by Snake Naive and Experienced California Ground Squirrels in a Simulated Burrow , 1978, Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie.

[22]  M. J. O'farrell Spatial Relationships of Rodents in a Sagebrush Community , 1980 .

[23]  R. Wiley,et al.  Mechanisms and Evolution of Spacing in Animals , 1979 .

[24]  G. Keppel,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 1976 .

[25]  K. Geluso,et al.  Spatial Distribution of Dipodomys spectabilis Mounds , 1975 .

[26]  D. Madison Space use and social structure in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus , 1980, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[27]  G. Kenagy Field Observations of Male Fighting, Drumming, and Copulation in the Great Basin Kangaroo Rat, Dipodomys microps , 1976 .

[28]  C. Krebs,et al.  Experimental alteration of sex ratios in populations of Microtus townsendii, a field vole , 1978 .

[29]  R. Holdenried Natural History of the Bannertail Kangaroo Rat in New Mexico , 1957 .

[30]  Philip N. Lehner,et al.  Handbook of ethological methods , 1979 .

[31]  James H. Brown,et al.  Granivory in Desert Ecosystems , 1979 .

[32]  D. Webster,et al.  Morphological Adaptations of the Ear in the Rodent Family Heteromyidae , 1980 .

[33]  J. Eisenberg The behavior of heteromyid rodents , 1963 .

[34]  J. Ferron Comfort behavior of the red squirrel (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus). , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.