The cognitive substrate of subjective probability.

The prominent cognitive theories of probability judgment were primarily developed to explain cognitive biases rather than to account for the cognitive processes in probability judgment. In this article the authors compare 3 major theories of the processes and representations in probability judgment: the representativeness heuristic, implemented as prototype similarity, relative likelihood, or evidential support accumulation (ESAM; D. J. Koehler, C. M. White, & R. Grondin, 2003); cue-based relative frequency; and exemplar memory, implemented by probabilities from exemplars (PROBEX; P. Juslin & M. Persson, 2002). Three experiments with different task structures consistently demonstrate that exemplar memory is the best account of the data whereas the results are inconsistent with extant formulations of the representativeness heuristic and cue-based relative frequency.

[1]  Jack B. Soll Determinants of Overconfidence and Miscalibration: The Roles of Random Error and Ecological Structure☆ , 1996 .

[2]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. , 2002 .

[3]  Mats Björkman,et al.  Brunswikian and Thurstonian Origins of Bias in Probability Assessment: On the Interpretation of Stochastic Components of Judgment , 1997 .

[4]  P. Juslin,et al.  Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect. , 2000, Psychological review.

[5]  Tilmann Betsch,et al.  Etc. frequency processing and cognition , 2002 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment , 1983 .

[7]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[8]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The Handbook of Child Language , 1995 .

[9]  Derek J. Koehler,et al.  An evidential support accumulation model of subjective probability , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[11]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[12]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[13]  Henrik Olsson,et al.  Exemplars, prototypes, and the flexibility of classification models. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Donald Homa,et al.  Abstraction of ill-defined form. , 1978 .

[15]  Magnus Persson,et al.  PROBabilities from EXemplars (PROBEX): a "lazy" algorithm for probabilistic inference from generic knowledge , 2002 .

[16]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Frequency processing: A twenty-five year perspective. , 2002 .

[17]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  "Schema Abstraction" in a Multiple-Trace Memory Model , 1986 .

[18]  P. Todd,et al.  Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .

[19]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .

[20]  Winston R. Sieck,et al.  Overconfidence effects in category learning: a comparison of connectionist and exemplar memory models. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  P. Juslin The Overconfidence Phenomenon as a Consequence of Informal Experimenter-Guided Selection of Almanac Items , 1994 .

[22]  M. Dougherty,et al.  Integration of the ecological and error models of overconfidence using a multiple-trace memory model. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  Mark A. Pitt,et al.  Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. , 2002 .

[24]  C. Gettys,et al.  MINERVA-DM: A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. , 1999 .

[25]  P. Juslin,et al.  Exemplar effects in categorization and multiple-cue judgment. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  P. Harris,et al.  When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[27]  D Kahneman,et al.  On the reality of cognitive illusions. , 1996, Psychological review.

[28]  D. Homa,et al.  Limitations of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. , 1981 .

[29]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Probabilistic mental models: a Brunswikian theory of confidence. , 1991, Psychological review.

[30]  Robert M. Nosofsky,et al.  Exemplar and prototype models revisited: response strategies, selective attention, and stimulus generalization. , 2002 .

[31]  L. Beach,et al.  Man as an Intuitive Statistician , 2022 .

[32]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[33]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Comparisons between exemplar similarity and mixed prototype models using a linearly separable category structure , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Stan A. Kuczaj,et al.  The Development of Word Meaning , 1986 .

[35]  A. Markman,et al.  Category use and category learning. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  Martyn D. Barrett,et al.  Early Semantic Representations and Early Word-Usage , 1986 .

[37]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .

[38]  W. Salmon The foundations of scientific inference , 1967 .

[39]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  How to Improve Bayesian Reasoning Without Instruction: Frequency Formats , 1995 .

[40]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[41]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Thirty categorization results in search of a model. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Cognition as Intuitive Statistics , 1987 .

[43]  N. Sanders,et al.  Journal of behavioral decision making: "The need for contextual and technical knowledge in judgmental forecasting", 5 (1992) 39-52 , 1992 .

[44]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Combining exemplar-based category representations and connectionist learning rules. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  Danielle R.M. Timmermans,et al.  The impact of task complexity on information use in multi‐attribute decision making , 1993 .

[46]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Prototypes in the Mist: The Early Epochs of Category Learning , 1998 .

[47]  Winston R. Sieck,et al.  Effects of Choice and Relative Frequency Elicitation on Overconfidence: Further Tests of an Exemplar-retrieval Model , 2003 .

[48]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Prototypes in category learning: the effects of category size, category structure, and stimulus complexity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[50]  K. Meints,et al.  Typicality, naming, and category membership in young children , 2001 .

[51]  W. Estes The cognitive side of probability learning. , 1976 .

[52]  B. Brehmer The psychology of linear judgement models , 1994 .

[53]  D. Mandel,et al.  The inverse fallacy: An account of deviations from Bayes’s theorem and the additivity principle , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[54]  J. Koehler The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Descriptive, normative, and methodological challenges , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[55]  R. Nosofsky Exemplars, prototypes, and similarity rules. , 1992 .

[56]  Mark K. Johansen,et al.  Exemplar-based accounts of "multiple-system" phenomena in perceptual categorization. , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.