Finding faces, animals, and vehicles in far peripheral vision.

Neuroimaging studies have shown that faces exhibit a central visual field bias, as compared to buildings and scenes. With a saccadic choice task, Crouzet, Kirchner, and Thorpe (2010) demonstrated a speed advantage for the detection of faces with stimuli located 8° from fixation. We used the same paradigm to examine whether the face advantage, relative to other categories (animals and vehicles), extends across the whole visual field (from 10° to 80° eccentricity) or whether it is limited to the central visual field. Pairs of photographs of natural scenes (a target and a distractor) were displayed simultaneously left and right of central fixation for 1s on a panoramic screen. Participants were asked to saccade to a target stimulus (faces, animals, or vehicles). The distractors were images corresponding to the two other categories. Eye movements were recorded with a head-mounted eye tracker. Only the first saccade was measured. Experiment 1 showed that (a) in terms of speed of categorization, faces maintain their advantage over animals and vehicles across the whole visual field, up to 80° and (b) even in crowded conditions (an object embedded in a scene), performance was above chance for the three categories of stimuli at 80° eccentricity. Experiment 2 showed that, when compared to another category with a high degree of within category structural similarity (cars), faces keep their advantage at all eccentricities. These results suggest that the bias for faces is not limited to the central visual field, at least in a categorization task.

[1]  Minami Ito,et al.  Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  D. Pelli Crowding: a cortical constraint on object recognition , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  The contributions of central versus peripheral vision to scene gist recognition. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[4]  Marie-Anne Hénaff,et al.  Emotional Facial Expression Detection in the Peripheral Visual Field , 2011, PloS one.

[5]  Jesse S. Husk,et al.  Spatial scaling factors explain eccentricity effects on face ERPs. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[6]  R. Rosenholtz,et al.  A summary statistic representation in peripheral vision explains visual search. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[7]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.

[8]  Olivier R. Joubert,et al.  The Time-Course of Visual Categorizations: You Spot the Animal Faster than the Bird , 2009, PloS one.

[9]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Functional Architecture for Disparity in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex and Its Relationship to the Architecture for Faces, Color, Scenes, and Visual Field , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Michèle Fabre-Thorpe,et al.  At 120 msec You Can Spot the Animal but You Don't Yet Know It's a Dog , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  E. Castet,et al.  Small effect of interline spacing on maximal reading speed in low-vision patients with central field loss irrespective of scotoma size. , 2010, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[12]  J. Rovamo,et al.  An estimation and application of the human cortical magnification factor , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  P. Artes,et al.  Face recognition in age related macular degeneration: perceived disability, measured disability, and performance with a bioptic device , 2002, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[14]  M D Anes,et al.  Roles of object-file review and type priming in visual identification within and across eye fixations. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  E. McKone,et al.  Isolating the special component of face recognition: peripheral identification and a Mooney face. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  S. Klein,et al.  Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification , 1985, Vision Research.

[17]  Muriel Boucart,et al.  Colour recognition at large visual eccentricities in normal observers and patients with low vision , 2006, Neuroreport.

[18]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Detection of animals in natural images using far peripheral vision , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  Kewei Chen,et al.  Regional Neural Response Differences in the Determination of Faces or Houses Positioned in a Wide Visual Field , 2013, PloS one.

[20]  Sébastien M. Crouzet,et al.  Fast saccades toward faces: face detection in just 100 ms. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[21]  Y. Yamane,et al.  Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Sensitivity to spatiotemporal colour contrast in the peripheral visual field , 1983, Vision Research.

[23]  D. Levi Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review , 2008, Vision Research.

[24]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Effect of distracting faces on visual selective attention in the monkey , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  N. Lavie,et al.  The Role of Perceptual Load in Processing Distractor Faces , 2003, Psychological science.

[26]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Automaticity of basic-level categorization accounts for labeling effects in visual recognition memory. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  I. Rentschler,et al.  Peripheral vision and pattern recognition: a review. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[28]  H Strasburger,et al.  Cortical Magnification Theory Fails to Predict Visual Recognition , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  M. Fabre-Thorpe,et al.  Implicit and explicit object recognition at very large visual eccentricities: No improvement after loss of central vision , 2010 .

[30]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—II. Low vision , 1985, Vision Research.

[31]  I L Bailey,et al.  Face recognition in age-related maculopathy. , 1991, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[32]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  M. Cannon,et al.  Perceived contrast in the fovea and periphery. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[34]  Jyrki Rovamo,et al.  Identification of facial images in peripheral vision , 2001, Vision Research.

[35]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  J. Moreland,et al.  Peripheral Colour Vision , 1972 .

[37]  Michelle R. Greene,et al.  Recognition of natural scenes from global properties: Seeing the forest without representing the trees , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  K. Rayner,et al.  Eye movements and word skipping during reading: effects of word length and predictability. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[40]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  At first sight: A high-level pop out effect for faces , 2005, Vision Research.

[41]  N. Kanwisher Faces and places: of central (and peripheral) interest , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  The wide window of face detection. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[43]  S. Hochstein,et al.  View from the Top Hierarchies and Reverse Hierarchies in the Visual System , 2002, Neuron.

[44]  M. Boucart,et al.  Implicit processing of scene context in macular degeneration. , 2013, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[45]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[46]  Bhuvanesh Awasthi,et al.  Processing of low spatial frequency faces at periphery in choice reaching tasks , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Muriel Boucart,et al.  Face or building superiority in peripheral vision reversed by task requirements , 2009, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[48]  M. Tovée,et al.  Translation invariance in the responses to faces of single neurons in the temporal visual cortical areas of the alert macaque. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  K. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Color perception in the intermediate periphery of the visual field. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[50]  F. Vitu,et al.  The role of object affordances and center of gravity in eye movements toward isolated daily-life objects. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[51]  Michèle Fabre-Thorpe,et al.  Stimulus duration and diversity do not reverse the advantage for superordinate‐level representations: the animal is seen before the bird , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[52]  D. Pelli,et al.  Are faces processed like words? A diagnostic test for recognition by parts. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[53]  S. Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Visual object understanding , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  Thomas Serre,et al.  Robust Object Recognition with Cortex-Like Mechanisms , 2007, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[56]  J. Findlay,et al.  Rapid Detection of Person Information in a Naturalistic Scene , 2008, Perception.