Cognitive Decline Affects Subject Attrition in Longitudinal Research

We evaluated prospectively 210 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (PD) to determine whether cognitive deterioration and disease disability affect subject drop out. Subjects who refused to return for follow-up testing had a greater degree of bradykinesia and overall disability, more advanced disease, fewer years of education and greater depressive symptomatology. However, discriminant analysis indicated that performance on the neuropsychological measures, rather than PD severity, significantly predicted whether patients return for follow-up testing. Our findings indicate that cognitive impairment uniquely contributes to subject attrition, which may distort dementia estimates in PD.

[1]  W. Weiner,et al.  Cognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson's disease , 1989, Neurology.

[2]  P. Laursen The impact of aging on cognitive functions. An 11 year follow-up study of four age cohorts. , 1997, Acta neurologica Scandinavica. Supplementum.

[3]  H. Weingartner,et al.  Cognitive impairments in Parkinson's disease: Distinguishing between effort-demanding and automatic cognitive processes , 1984, Psychiatry Research.

[4]  B. Ratcliff,et al.  Handbook of Parkinson's Disease , 1988, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[5]  E. Sullivan,et al.  Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  J. Lindeboom,et al.  Low education is a genuine risk factor for accelerated memory decline and dementia. , 1997, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[7]  D. Delis,et al.  Are all subcortical dementias alike? Verbal learning and memory in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease patients. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[8]  Y. Stern,et al.  The effect of patient attrition on estimates of the frequency of dementia following stroke. , 1998, Archives of neurology.

[9]  J. W. Warburton Memory disturbance and the Parkinson syndrome. , 1967, The British journal of medical psychology.

[10]  H. Teräväinen,et al.  The effect of age of disease onset on neuropsychological performance in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  J. Tobin,et al.  Factors affecting attrition in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging , 1986, Experimental Gerontology.

[12]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Dementia in Parkinson's disease , 1994, Neurology.

[13]  N. Lambert,et al.  Tracking procedures and attrition containment in a long-term follow-up of a community-based ADHD sample. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[14]  D P Salmon,et al.  The Mini-Mental State Examination in the early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[15]  W. Eaton,et al.  Psychopathology and attrition in the Baltimore ECA 15-year follow-up 1981–1996 , 1999, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.

[16]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  W. Weiner,et al.  Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson's disease , 1991, Neurology.

[18]  C. Marsden,et al.  The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of Parkinson's disease , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  H. Katzen,et al.  Age of disease onset influences cognition in Parkinson's disease , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[20]  E. Crimmins,et al.  Loss to folow-up in a sample of Americans 70 years of age and older: The LSOA 1984-1990. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[21]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[22]  Ruth B. Ekstrom,et al.  Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests , 1976 .

[23]  R. Knight The Neuropsychology of Degenerative Brain Diseases , 1992 .

[24]  Judith L. Navratil,et al.  Minimizing subject loss in a longitudinal study of deviant behavior , 1994 .

[25]  N. Day,et al.  Retention of resistant subjects in longitudinal studies: description and procedures. , 1994, The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse.

[26]  S Corkin,et al.  Cognitive impairments in Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Advances in neurology.

[27]  K. Marder,et al.  A population-based investigation of Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Relationship to age and gender. , 1992, Archives of neurology.

[28]  A. Lees,et al.  Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  J. Burg,et al.  The Potential for Bias Due to Attrition in the national Exposure Registry: An Examination of Reasons for Nonresponse, Nonrespondent Characteristics, and the Response Rate , 1997, Toxicology and industrial health.

[30]  E. Feskens,et al.  Non-response bias in a study of cardiovascular diseases, functional status and self-rated health among elderly men. , 1998, Age and ageing.

[31]  M. Kuskowski,et al.  Dementia in Parkinson disease: A neuropsychological analysis , 1982, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  F. Boller,et al.  Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson's disease. Role of perceptual and motor factors. , 1984, Archives of neurology.