A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention

[1]  Christopher L Asplund,et al.  Surprise-induced blindness: a stimulus-driven attentional limit to conscious perception. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  S. Yantis,et al.  A Domain-Independent Source of Cognitive Control for Task Sets: Shifting Spatial Attention and Switching Categorization Rules , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Representation of Eye Movements and Stimulus Motion in Topographically Organized Areas of Human Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[5]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  E. Miller,et al.  Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices , 2007, Science.

[8]  Biyu J. He,et al.  Breakdown of Functional Connectivity in Frontoparietal Networks Underlies Behavioral Deficits in Spatial Neglect , 2007, Neuron.

[9]  J. Gottlieb From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition , 2007, Neuron.

[10]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Space and the parietal cortex , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[12]  R. Marois,et al.  Visual Short-Term Memory Load Suppresses Temporo-Parietal Junction Activity and Induces Inattentional Blindness , 2005, Psychological science.

[13]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  R. Marois,et al.  Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.

[17]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Rapid Prefrontal-Hippocampal Habituation to Novel Events , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  M. Chun,et al.  The Neural Fate of Consciously Perceived and Missed Events in the Attentional Blink , 2004, Neuron.

[19]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[20]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .

[21]  S. Yantis,et al.  Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Correlations and dissociations between BOLD signal and P300 amplitude in an auditory oddball task: a parametric approach to combining fMRI and ERP. , 2002, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[23]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of consciousness in humans , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Dissociating the Neural Mechanisms of Visual Attention in Change Detection Using Functional MRI , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Maliha S. Nash,et al.  Handbook of Parametric and Nonparametric Statistical Procedures , 2001, Technometrics.

[27]  C. Frith,et al.  Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Gore,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Attentional Blink , 2000, Neuron.

[29]  Debashis Kushary,et al.  Bootstrap Methods and Their Application , 2000, Technometrics.

[30]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Gore,et al.  A Stimulus-Driven Approach to Object Identity and Location Processing in the Human Brain , 2000, Neuron.

[33]  C. Frith,et al.  Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. , 1999, Science.

[34]  R. Goebel,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  A. Friederici,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of novelty processing: integrating ERP and fMRI results. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[38]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[39]  Anthony C. Davison,et al.  Bootstrap Methods and Their Application , 1998 .

[40]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[42]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[45]  J. Downar,et al.  A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  D. Sheskin Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures, 2nd ed. , 2000 .

[48]  B. Baars IN THE THEATRE OF CONSCIOUSNESS Global Workspace Theory, A Rigorous Scientific Theory of Consciousness. , 1997 .

[49]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[50]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[51]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[52]  K M Heilman,et al.  Mechanisms underlying the unilateral neglect syndrome. , 1977, Advances in neurology.