Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces.

Researchers of visual perception have long been interested in the perceived slant of a surface and in the gradients that purportedly specify it. Slant is the angle between the line of sight and the tangent to the planar surface at any point, also called the surface normal. Gradients are the sources of information that grade, or change, with visual angle as one looks from one's feet upward to the horizon. The present article explores three gradients--perspective, compression, and density--and the phenomenal impression of flat and curved surfaces. The perspective gradient is measured at right angles to the axis of tilt at any point in the optic array; that is, when looking down a hallway at the tiles of a floor receding in the distance, perspective is measured by the x-axis width of each tile projected on the image plane orthogonal to the line of sight. The compression gradient is the ratio of y/x axis measures on the projected plane. The density gradient is measured by the number of tiles per unit solid visual angle. For flat surfaces and many others, perspective and compression gradients decrease with distance, and the density gradient increases. We discuss the manner in which these gradients change for various types of surfaces. Each gradient is founded on a different assumption about textures on the surfaces around us. In Experiment 1, viewers assessed the three-dimensional character of projections of flat and curved surfaces receding in the distance. They made pairwise judgments of preference and of dissimilarity among eight stimuli in each of four sets. The presence of each gradient was manipulated orthogonally such that each stimulus had zero, one, two, or three gradients appropriate for either a flat surface or a curved surface. Judgments were made were made for surfaces with both regularly shaped and irregularly shaped textures scattered on them. All viewer assessment were then scaled in one dimension. Multiple correlation and regression on the scale values revealed that greater than 98% of the variance in scale values was accounted for by the gradients. For the flat surfaces a mean of 65% of the variance was accounted for by the perspective gradient, 28% by the density gradient, and 6% by the compression gradient. For curved surfaces, on the other hand, a mean of 96% of the variance was accounted for by the compression gradient, and less than 2% by either the perspective gradient or the density gradient.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)

[1]  J. Gibson,et al.  Does motion perspective independently produce the impression of a receding surface? , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[3]  J. Gibson,et al.  The relation of apparent shape to apparent slant in the perception of objects. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  W. Clark,et al.  The interaction of surface texture, outline gradient, and ground in the perception of slant. , 1956, Canadian journal of psychology.

[5]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[6]  W. Epstein,et al.  The induction of nonveridical slant and the perception of shape. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  H R FLOCK,et al.  A POSSIBLE OPTICAL BASIS FOR MONOCULAR SLANT PERCEPTION. , 1964, Psychological review.

[8]  F. Attneave,et al.  Inferences about visual mechanisms from monocular depth effects , 1966 .

[9]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  Absolute threshold for visual slant: the effect of stimulus size and retinal perspective. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  H. Flock,et al.  Slant judgments of single rectangles at a slant , 1967 .

[11]  J. Payne,et al.  Perspective and form ratio as determinants of relative slant judgments , 1969 .

[12]  B Gillam,et al.  Judgments of slant on the basis of foreshortening. , 1970, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[13]  R. Phillips Stationary Visual Texture and the Estimation of Slant Angle , 1970, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  D. Vickers Perceptual economy and the impression of visual depth , 1971 .

[15]  R. Shepard Representation of structure in similarity data: Problems and prospects , 1974 .

[16]  R. Olson Slant judgments from static and rotating trapezoids correspond to rules of perspective geometry , 1974 .

[17]  Berthold K. P. Horn Obtaining shape from shading information , 1989 .

[18]  R. R. Rosinski,et al.  Texture gradient effectiveness in the perception of surface slant. , 1976, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

[19]  Ruzena Bajcsy,et al.  Texture gradient as a depth cue , 1976 .

[20]  M. Braunstein Depth perception through motion , 1976 .

[21]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  John C. Baird,et al.  Fundamentals of scaling and psychophysics , 1978 .

[23]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[24]  S. Ullman,et al.  The interpretation of visual motion , 1977 .

[25]  J. Perrone Slant Underestimation: A Model Based on the Size of the Viewing Aperture , 1980, Perception.

[26]  Andrew P. Witkin,et al.  Recovering Surface Shape and Orientation from Texture , 1981, Artif. Intell..

[27]  Kent A. Stevens,et al.  The Visual Interpretation of Surface Contours , 1981, Artif. Intell..

[28]  D J McArthur,et al.  Computer vision and perceptual psychology. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  J. Todd Visual information about rigid and nonrigid motion: a geometric analysis. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  K. A. Stevens Surface tilt (the direction of slant): A neglected psychophysical variable , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  J. R. Pomerantz Global and local precedence: selective attention in form and motion perception. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  E. Cutting,et al.  Observations Four Assumptions About Invariance in Perception , 1983 .

[33]  E. Mingolla,et al.  Perception of surface curvature and direction of illumination from patterns of shading. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.