Basics for sensorimotor information processing: some implications for learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B. Burle,et al. Action Monitoring and Medial Frontal Cortex: Leading Role of Supplementary Motor Area , 2014, Science.
[2] J. Gordon,et al. Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[4] Steven W. Keele,et al. Attention and human performance , 1973 .
[5] Stephen J. Payne,et al. Naive Judgments of Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1995, Hum. Factors.
[6] C. Braun,et al. Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] Shaffer Lh,et al. Some effects of partial advance information on choice reaction with fixed or variable S-R mapping. , 1966 .
[9] Borís Burle,et al. Tactile stimulations and wheel rotation responses: toward augmented lane departure warning systems , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[10] A. P. Georgopoulos,et al. Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.
[11] Maxime J Parent,et al. Movement chunking during sequence learning is a dopamine-dependant process: a study conducted in Parkinson’s disease , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[13] G. Smith,et al. Slowness and age: speed-accuracy mechanisms. , 1995, Psychology and aging.
[14] Ken Nakayama,et al. Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions , 1986, Nature.
[15] P. Rabbitt. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[16] Joan E. Regan. Automaticity and Learning: Effects of Familiarity on Naming Letters. , 1981 .
[17] M Akamatsu,et al. Changes in spinal excitability during choice reaction time: the H reflex as a probe of information transmission. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[18] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[19] A. Sanders. Issues and trends in the debate on discrete vs. continuous processing of information , 1990 .
[20] P Bertelson,et al. The Time Course of Preparation* , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] S Kornblum,et al. Dynamics of Single Neuron Activity in Monkey Primary Motor Cortex Related to Sensorimotor Transformation , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[22] A. Riehle,et al. Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] S. T. Klapp,et al. Response programming in simple and choice reactions. , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.
[24] Carl Philipp Emanuel Bach,et al. Essay on the True Art of Playing Keyboard Instruments , 1948 .
[25] T Hasbroucq,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: toward a conceptual clarification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Testing theories of post-error slowing , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[27] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[28] Birte U. Forstmann,et al. How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution , 2012 .
[29] Jane E. Clark,et al. Development of state estimation explains improvements in sensorimotor performance across childhood. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[31] S. T. Klapp,et al. Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. , 1995 .
[32] Steven W. Keele,et al. Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .
[33] W A MacKay,et al. CNV, stretch reflex and reaction time correlates of preparation for movement direction and force. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[34] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled & automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[35] Franck Vidal,et al. The N-40: An electrophysiological marker of response selection , 2013, Biological Psychology.
[36] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[37] A. Treisman. The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[38] R. Pew. The speed-accuracy operating characteristic , 1969 .
[39] Franck Vidal,et al. The dual nature of time preparation: neural activation and suppression revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.
[40] R. Proctor,et al. Naïve and experienced judgments of stimuluS—Response compatibility: implications for interface design , 2003, Ergonomics.
[41] C Ghez,et al. Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.
[42] D. Norman,et al. Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .
[43] A. Riehle. Visually induced signal-locked neuronal activity changes in precentral motor areas of the monkey: hierarchical progression of signal processing , 1991, Brain Research.
[44] D. Glencross,et al. Some Experimental Evidence For and Against a Parametric Conception of Movement Programming , 1989 .
[45] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[46] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .
[47] M. Posner,et al. Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.
[48] James L. McClelland,et al. Hick's law in a stochastic race model with speed-accuracy tradeoffs , 2002 .
[49] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[50] Alexa Riehle,et al. Preparation for Action : one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .
[51] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[52] T Hasbroucq,et al. The influence of time preparation on motor processes assessed by surface Laplacian estimation , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[53] W. E. Hick. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.
[54] Borís Burle,et al. Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.
[55] L. Robertson. Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[56] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[57] Franck Vidal,et al. An ERP study of cognitive architecture and the insertion of mental processes: Donders revisited. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[58] H. Woodrow. The measurement of attention , 1914 .
[59] M. Zorzi,et al. The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[60] P. Fitts. Cognitive aspects of information processing. 3. Set for speed versus accuracy. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[61] R. Shiffrin,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .
[62] Daniel Gopher,et al. On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .
[63] P. Cavanagh,et al. Flexible cognitive resources: competitive content maps for attention and memory , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[64] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[65] Wim Fias,et al. Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.
[66] Stefaan Vandorpe,et al. Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Multiple resources and performance prediction , 2002 .
[68] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Multiple Resources and Mental Workload , 2008, Hum. Factors.
[69] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[70] Errol R Hoffmann,et al. Naïve judgements of stimulus–response compatibility , 2010, Ergonomics.
[71] F. Vidal,et al. Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[72] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[73] H Shibasaki,et al. Movement-related potentials associated with bilateral simultaneous and unilateral movements recorded from human supplementary motor area. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[74] R. Bogacz,et al. The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.
[75] Peter Vuust,et al. To musicians, the message is in the meter: Pre-attentive neuronal responses to incongruent rhythm are left-lateralized in musicians , 2005, NeuroImage.
[76] M. W. Molen,et al. Lifespan changes in motor activation and inhibition during choice reactions: A Laplacian ERP study , 2012, Biological Psychology.
[77] Franck Vidal,et al. Programming the duration of a motor sequence: role of the primary and supplementary motor areas in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[78] P. Viviani,et al. Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[79] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[80] A. Treisman,et al. Perceiving visually presented objets: recognition, awareness, and modularity , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[81] E. Donchin,et al. Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing: an analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[82] S. Hochstein,et al. View from the Top Hierarchies and Reverse Hierarchies in the Visual System , 2002, Neuron.
[83] Stephen Monsell,et al. The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting , 1978 .
[84] J. Davies,et al. Molecular Biology of the Cell , 1983, Bristol Medico-Chirurgical Journal.
[85] Baptist Liefooghe,et al. Eliminating the Simon effect by instruction. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[86] A. Nobre,et al. Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[87] H N Zelaznik,et al. Reaction time methods in the study of motor programming: the precuing of hand, digit, and duration. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.
[88] R. Hyman. Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[89] Hartmut Leuthold,et al. Planning of rapid aiming movements and the contingent negative variation: are movement duration and extent specified independently? , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[90] Marco Zorzi,et al. Cross-modal re-mapping influences the Simon effect , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[91] Alexa Riehle,et al. Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .
[92] J. Edwards,et al. Motor sequence chunking is impaired by basal ganglia stroke , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[93] Franck Vidal,et al. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of information processing in the motor cortex: relationship between the silent period and the reaction time delay. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[94] Daniel G Bobrow,et al. On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.
[95] Franck Vidal,et al. Orienting Attention in Time Activates Left Intraparietal Sulcus for Both Perceptual and Motor Task Goals , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[96] Borís Burle,et al. How does temporal preparation speed up response implementation in choice tasks? Evidence for an early cortical activation. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[97] Y. Guiard,et al. Principles of response determination: The list-rule model of SR compatibility , 1990 .
[98] Franck Vidal,et al. On-line executive control: an electromyographic study. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[99] L H Shaffer,et al. Some effects of partial advance information on choice reaction with fixed or variable S-R mapping. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[100] E. Miller,et al. Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.
[101] Elger L. Abrahamse,et al. Post-error slowing in sequential action: an aging study , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[102] D. Hubel,et al. Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.
[103] S. Kastner,et al. Neuronal Correlates of Pop-out in Cat Striate Cortex , 1997, Vision Research.
[104] Franck Vidal,et al. Sequential adjustments before and after partial errors , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[105] Stuart T. Klapp,et al. Syllable-dependent pronunciation latencies in number naming: A replication. , 1974 .
[106] Claude Prablanc,et al. Is there an optimal arm posture? Deterioration of finger localization precision and comfort sensation in extreme arm-joint postures , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[107] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .
[108] Franck Vidal,et al. The nature of unilateral motor commands in between-hand choice tasks as revealed by surface Laplacian estimation. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[109] Hartmut Leuthold,et al. Short article: Control over speeded actions: A common processing locus for micro- and macro-trade-offs? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[110] D. Rosenbaum. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[111] Franck Vidal,et al. Motor inhibition and response expectancy: A Laplacian ERP study , 2010, Biological Psychology.
[112] O. Hikosaka,et al. Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.