The Cost of Switching between Kanji and Kana while
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] John Macnamara,et al. Linguistic independence of bilinguals: The input switch , 1971 .
[2] S. Los,et al. On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .
[3] L. Colombo. Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. , 1986 .
[4] Paul W. B. Atkins,et al. Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .
[5] I. Biederman,et al. Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .
[6] T Hirose,et al. [The effect of script frequency on semantic processing of Kanji and Kana words]. , 1984, Shinrigaku kenkyu : The Japanese journal of psychology.
[7] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[8] Competitor priming in spoken word recognition. , 1998 .
[9] D. LaBerge. Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] Naoyuki Osaka,et al. Eye fixation and saccade during kana and kanji text reading: Comparison of English and Japanese text processing. , 1989 .
[11] Derek Besner,et al. Role of set in visual word recognition: Activation and activation blocking as nonautomatic processes. , 1996 .
[12] S. Monsell,et al. Lexical and sublexical translation of spelling to sound : Strategic anticipation of lexical status , 1992 .
[13] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[14] G. Humphreys,et al. Phonologically mediated access to meaning for Kanji: Is a rows still a rose in Japanese Kanji? , 1993 .
[15] Derek Besner,et al. The stroop effect and the myth of automaticity , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[16] Hirofumi Saito. Use of graphemic and phonemic encoding in reading Kanji and Kana , 1981 .
[17] T H Carr,et al. Automaticity and cognitive anatomy: is word recognition "automatic"? , 1992, The American journal of psychology.
[18] G. C. Orden,et al. Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Michael T. Turvey,et al. Words Written in Kana Are Named Faster Than the Same Words Written in Kanji , 1980 .
[20] K. Forster,et al. Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .
[21] J. Grainger,et al. Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990 .
[22] A Nakagawa. Visual and semantic processing in reading Kanji. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] J. H. Neely. Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .
[24] Karl J. Friston,et al. Studying brain function with neuroimaging , 1997 .
[25] Karalyn Patterson,et al. Interpreting a Case of Japanese Phonological Alexia: The Key is in Phonology , 1996 .
[26] J. Grainger,et al. Language blocking and lexical access in bilinguals , 1987 .
[27] Derek Besner,et al. Orthographic and phonological codes in the oral reading of Japanese Kana. , 1987 .
[28] D. Allport,et al. What Are the Functional Orthographic Units in Chinese Word Recognition: The Stroke or the Stroke Pattern? , 1996 .
[29] C. Eriksen,et al. Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] T. Hatta. RECOGNITION OF JAPANESE KANJI AND HIRAKANA IN THE LEFT AND RIGHT VISUAL FIELDS , 1978 .
[31] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[32] A. Caramazza,et al. Semantic classification by bilinguals. , 1980 .
[33] S. Monsell,et al. Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .
[34] G. Humphreys,et al. The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .
[35] Randolph G. Bias,et al. Phonological recoding and reading. , 1981 .
[36] S. Lupker,et al. Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .
[37] K. Rayner,et al. The psychology of reading , 1989 .
[38] A. Marcel. Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.
[39] Brian Butterworth,et al. The inconsistency of consistency effects in reading: the case of Japanese kanji phonology , 1994, ICSLP.
[40] Chris M. Herdman,et al. CODE-SPECIFIC PROCESSES IN WORD NAMING : EVIDENCE SUPPORTING A DUAL-ROUTE MODEL OF WORD RECOGNITION , 1996 .
[41] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[42] S. Sasanuma,et al. Progressive aphasia and surface Alexia in Japanese , 1995 .
[43] Yuko Kimura,et al. Concurrent Vocal Interference: Its Effects on Kana and Kanji , 1984 .
[44] S. Sasanuma,et al. The syndrome of Gogi (word‐meaning) aphasia , 1975, Neurology.
[45] A. Allport. Word Recognition in Reading (Tutorial Paper) , 1979 .
[46] Giovanni B. Flores d'Arcais,et al. PHONOLOGICAL AND SEMANTIC ACTIVATION IN READING KANJI CHARACTERS , 1995 .
[47] Raja Parasuraman,et al. SuperLab: General-purpose Macintosh software for human experimental psychology and psychological testing , 1993 .
[48] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.