Rhythm synchronization performance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians

Behavioural and neuroimaging studies provide evidence for a possible “sensitive” period in childhood development during which musical training results in long-lasting changes in brain structure and auditory and motor performance. Previous work from our laboratory has shown that adult musicians who begin training before the age of 7 (early-trained; ET) perform better on a visuomotor task than those who begin after the age of 7 (late-trained; LT), even when matched on total years of musical training and experience. Two questions were raised regarding the findings from this experiment. First, would this group performance difference be observed using a more familiar, musically relevant task such as auditory rhythms? Second, would cognitive abilities mediate this difference in task performance? To address these questions, ET and LT musicians, matched on years of musical training, hours of current practice and experience, were tested on an auditory rhythm synchronization task. The task consisted of six woodblock rhythms of varying levels of metrical complexity. In addition, participants were tested on cognitive subtests measuring vocabulary, working memory and pattern recognition. The two groups of musicians differed in their performance of the rhythm task, such that the ET musicians were better at reproducing the temporal structure of the rhythms. There were no group differences on the cognitive measures. Interestingly, across both groups, individual task performance correlated with auditory working memory abilities and years of formal training. These results support the idea of a sensitive period during the early years of childhood for developing sensorimotor synchronization abilities via musical training.

[1]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Cerebellar volume of musicians. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  V. Menon,et al.  White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of musicianship as revealed by cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  P. Essens,et al.  Structuring temporal sequences: Comparison of models and factors of complexity , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition , 2008 .

[6]  I. Peretz,et al.  Music, language and cognition: unresolved issues , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  G. Schlaug The Brain of Musicians , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[8]  E. Knudsen Sensitive Periods in the Development of the Brain and Behavior , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Aaron J. Newman,et al.  A critical period for right hemisphere recruitment in American Sign Language processing , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Musical Training Shapes Structural Brain Development , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  J. J. Ryan,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .

[12]  Betty Ann Levy,et al.  Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  Bryan M. Hooks,et al.  Critical Periods in the Visual System: Changing Views for a Model of Experience-Dependent Plasticity , 2007, Neuron.

[14]  Thomas Rammsayer,et al.  Processing of temporal and nontemporal information as predictors of psychometric intelligence: a structural-equation-modeling approach. , 2007, Journal of personality.

[15]  Robert J Zatorre,et al.  Differences in Gray Matter between Musicians and Nonmusicians , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  J. Saffran,et al.  Musical Learning and Language Development , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  H. Neville,et al.  Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[18]  M. Svirsky,et al.  Development of Language and Speech Perception in Congenitally, Profoundly Deaf Children as a Function of Age at Cochlear Implantation , 2004, Audiology and Neurotology.

[19]  S. Curtiss Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day "Wild Child" , 1977 .

[20]  Stefan Skare,et al.  See Blockindiscussions, Blockinstats, Blockinand Blockinauthor Blockinprofiles Blockinfor Blockinthis Blockinpublication Extensive Blockinpiano Blockinpracticing Blockinhas Blockinregionally Specific Blockineffects Blockinon Blockinwhite Blockinmatter Blockindevelopment , 2022 .

[21]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Long-Term Positive Associations between Music Lessons and IQ. , 2006 .

[22]  Richard Lynn,et al.  Simple musical tests as measures of Spearman's g , 1989 .

[23]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Effects of Music Training on the Child's Brain and Cognitive Development , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians , 1998, Nature.

[25]  P. Essens,et al.  Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Eugenia Costa-Giomi Does Music Instruction Improve Fine Motor Abilities? , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  R. Zatorre,et al.  When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[28]  M. Bangert,et al.  Specialization of the specialized in features of external human brain morphology , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  D. Hubel,et al.  Extent of recovery from the effects of visual deprivation in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. , 1999, Science.

[31]  M. Besson,et al.  Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  C. Nelson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning in preschool- and school-age children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Brain Structures Differ between Musicians and Non-Musicians , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  A. H. Takeuchi,et al.  Absolute pitch. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  A. Schleicher,et al.  Motor cortex and hand motor skills: Structural compliance in the human brain , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Tal Savion-Lemieux,et al.  The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  M. Dorman,et al.  Deprivation-induced cortical reorganization in children with cochlear implants , 2007, International journal of audiology.

[38]  F. Linthicum,et al.  The human auditory system: A timeline of development , 2007, International journal of audiology.

[39]  J. Bruce Tomblin,et al.  Developmental constraints on language development in children with cochlear implants , 2007, International journal of audiology.

[40]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Arousal, Mood, and The Mozart Effect , 2001, Psychological science.

[41]  Tal Savion-Lemieux,et al.  Developmental contributions to motor sequence learning , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Guy Madison,et al.  Intelligence and Variability in a Simple Timing Task Share Neural Substrates in the Prefrontal White Matter , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Staiger,et al.  Increased corpus callosum size in musicians , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  G. Schlaug,et al.  Practicing a Musical Instrument in Childhood is Associated with Enhanced Verbal Ability and Nonverbal Reasoning , 2008, PloS one.

[45]  Thomas Rammsayer,et al.  Performance on temporal information processing as an index of general intelligence , 2007 .

[46]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Music Lessons Enhance IQ , 2004, Psychological science.

[47]  G. Innocenti Subcortical regulation of cortical development: some effects of early, selective deprivations. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[48]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps , 2000, Nature.

[49]  J. Bartko,et al.  Cortical correlates of neuromotor development in healthy children , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[50]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children , 2009, NeuroImage.

[51]  L. Trainor Are there critical periods for musical development? , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.

[52]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Moving on Time: Brain Network for Auditory-Motor Synchronization is Modulated by Rhythm Complexity and Musical Training , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Zatorre Absolute pitch: a model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Guy Madison,et al.  Correlations between intelligence and components of serial timing variability , 2008 .

[55]  Rainer Hartmann,et al.  Delayed Maturation and Sensitive Periods in the Auditory Cortex , 2001, Audiology and Neurotology.

[56]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Music and Nonmusical Abilities , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Gottfried Schlaug,et al.  Musicians Differ from Nonmusicians in Brain Activation despite Performance Matching , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[58]  E. Lenneberg Biological Foundations of Language , 1967 .

[59]  Antoine J. Shahin,et al.  Enhancement of auditory cortical development by musical experience in children , 2004, Neuroreport.