The impact of job demands and behavioral control on experienced job stress

The present study examines the impact of behavioral control on the experience of work strain under conditions of work overload. Extending experimental laboratory findings that suggest that control can lessen the impact of aversive stimuli on psychological and physiological strain responses, we hypothesized an interaction between control and workload such that the effects of high demands on strain would be less if the worker had behavioral control over the task. This hypothesis is also consistent with Karasek's job demands-job decision latitude model of work strain and health. The hypothesis was tested in a laboratory experiment in which 125 subjects worked on a mail sorting task with either a high or moderate level of workload and either a high or low level of behavioral control. Strain responses were assessed with measures of job satisfaction, anxiety, and physiological arousal. The hypothesis was only partially supported in that high control lessened the impact that work overload had on anxiety. While not fully supportive of the model, the results are seen as conservative given the constraints on the manipulation of work overload inherent in the laboratory environment.

[1]  R. Rich,et al.  SUITABILITY OF THE SUBJECTIVE STRESS SCALE FOR HOSPITALIZED SUBJECTS , 1969, Nursing research.

[2]  J. Rotter Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. , 1966, Psychological monographs.

[3]  John M. Ivancevich,et al.  Organizational Stressors and Heart Disease: A Research Model , 1979 .

[4]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Lack of control as a determinant of perceived physical symptoms. , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  J. Singer,et al.  Urban stress: experiments on noise and social stressors , 1972 .

[6]  S. Epstein,et al.  Availability of an avoidance response as related to autonomic arousal. , 1976, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[7]  M. Friedman,et al.  Changes in the Serum Cholesterol and Blood Clotting Time in Men Subjected to Cyclic Variation of Occupational Stress , 1958, Circulation.

[8]  D. G. Gardner ACTIVATION THEORY AND TASK DESIGN: DEVELOPMENT OF A CONCEPTUAL MODEL AND AN EMPIRICAL TEST , 1981 .

[9]  R. W. White Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. , 1959, Psychological review.

[10]  S. Thompson Will it hurt less if i can control it? A complex answer to a simple question. , 1981 .

[11]  D. S. Holmes,et al.  Behavior control over aversive events: does control that requires effort reduce anxiety and physiological arousal? , 1980, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Participation in Decision-Making: One More Look , 1979 .

[13]  Gary P. Latham,et al.  Effects of Assigned and Participative Goal Setting on Performance and Job Satisfaction. , 1976 .

[14]  Suzanne M. Miller,et al.  Controllability and human stress: method, evidence and theory. , 1979, Behaviour research and therapy.

[15]  S. Jackson Participation in decision making as a strategy for reducing job-related strain. , 1983 .

[16]  E. Langer,et al.  The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: a field experiment in an institutional setting. , 1976, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  M. Frankenhaeuser,et al.  Task demand as reflected in catecholamine excretion and heart rate. , 1976, Journal of human stress.

[18]  Lawrence R. Murphy,et al.  Occupational stress management: A review and appraisal , 1984 .

[19]  A Ahlbom,et al.  Job decision latitude, job demands, and cardiovascular disease: a prospective study of Swedish men. , 1981, American journal of public health.

[20]  Stephen M. Sales Organizational Role as a Risk Factor in Coronary Disease , 1973, IEEE Engineering Management Review.

[21]  J. Hokanson,et al.  Availability of avoidance behaviors in modulating vascular-stress responses. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Robert Karasek,et al.  Physiology of Stress and Regeneration in Job Related Cardiovascular Illness , 1982 .

[23]  J. Marshall,et al.  Occupational sources of stress ; a review of the literature relating to coronary heart disease and mental ill health , 1976 .

[24]  Theodore Kunin The Construction of a New Type of Attitude Measure , 1955 .

[25]  E. Stotland,et al.  THE REDUCTION OF ANXIETY AS A RESULT OF THE EXPECTATION OF MAKING A CHOICE. , 1964, Canadian journal of psychology.

[26]  Randall S. Schuler,et al.  Role Conflict and Role Ambiguity: Integration of the Literature and Directions for Future Research , 1981 .

[27]  Robert Karasek,et al.  Job decision latitude and mental strain: Implications for job redesign , 1979 .

[28]  John M. Ivancevich,et al.  Stress and work : a managerial perspective , 1980 .

[29]  Joseph J. Hurrell,et al.  Machine-paced work and the Type A behaviour pattern. , 1985 .

[30]  Bronston T. Mayes,et al.  Managing Organizational Stress: A Field Experiment , 1982 .

[31]  N. L. Corah,et al.  Perceived Control, Self- Observation, and Response to Aversive Stimulation. , 1970 .

[32]  Richard,et al.  Motivation through the Design of Work: Test of a Theory. , 1976 .

[33]  H. Selye The Stress of Life , 1958 .

[34]  R. Caplan,et al.  Effects of work load, role ambiguity, and type A personality on anxiety, depression, and heart rate. , 1975, The Journal of applied psychology.

[35]  J. Thibaut,et al.  The effects of control on perceived fairness of procedures and outcomes , 1983 .

[36]  J. Geer,et al.  Evaluating the effects of the prediction-control confound. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  M. Zuckerman The development of an affect adjective check list for the measurement of anxiety. , 1960, Journal of consulting psychology.