Enhanced detection of visual targets on the hand and familiar tools

[1]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Vision in the palm of your hand , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  A hand in blindsight: Hand placement near target improves size perception in the blind visual field , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  M. Goodale,et al.  Koniocellular projections and hand-assisted blindsight , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  A. Dufour,et al.  Improved visual sensitivity in the perihand space , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Is That Near My Hand? Multisensory Representation of Peripersonal Space in Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  S. Ichinose,et al.  Extension of Corticocortical Afferents into the Anterior Bank of the Intraparietal Sulcus by Tool-use Training in Adult Monkeys , 2005 .

[8]  Jefferson D. Grubb,et al.  Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  R. Passingham,et al.  That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.

[10]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Reaching Out to See: Arm Position Can Attenuate Human Visual Loss , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  P. Haggard,et al.  Facilitated Processing of Visual Stimuli Associated with the Body , 2004, Perception.

[12]  C. Spence,et al.  Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans , 2002, Cognition.

[13]  Jon Driver,et al.  Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  S. Jackson,et al.  ‘Action binding’: dynamic interactions between vision and touch , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  S. Tipper,et al.  Vision influences tactile perception at body sites that cannot be viewed directly , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  M S Graziano,et al.  Coding the location of the arm by sight. , 2000, Science.

[17]  Francesca Frassinetti,et al.  Direct evidence from parietal extinction of enhancement of visual attention near a visible hand , 2000, Current Biology.

[18]  M. Graziano,et al.  Location of the polysensory zone in the precentral gyrus of anesthetized monkeys , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  A Farnè,et al.  Dynamic size‐change of hand peripersonal space following tool use , 2000, Neuroreport.

[20]  M. Graziano Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  S. Tipper,et al.  Vision influences tactile perception without proprioceptive orienting , 1998, Neuroreport.

[22]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[23]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[24]  R. Hari,et al.  Preference of Personal to Extrapersonal Space in a Visuomotor Task , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[27]  C. Gross,et al.  A bimodal map of space: somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  P. Haggard,et al.  Visual processing and the bodily self. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[29]  R. Johansson,et al.  Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.