How Much Spatial Information Is Lost in the Sensory Substitution Process? Comparing Visual, Tactile, and Auditory Approaches

Sensory substitution devices (SSDs) can convey visuospatial information through spatialised auditory or tactile stimulation using wearable technology. However, the level of information loss associated with this transformation is unknown. In this study, novice users discriminated the location of two objects at 1.2 m using devices that transformed a 16 × 8-depth map into spatially distributed patterns of light, sound, or touch on the abdomen. Results showed that through active sensing, participants could discriminate the vertical position of objects to a visual angle of 1°, 14°, and 21°, and their distance to 2 cm, 8 cm, and 29 cm using these visual, auditory, and haptic SSDs, respectively. Visual SSDs significantly outperformed auditory and tactile SSDs on vertical localisation, whereas for depth perception, all devices significantly differed from one another (visual > auditory > haptic). Our findings highlight the high level of acuity possible for SSDs even with low spatial resolutions (e.g., 16 × 8) and quantify the level of information loss attributable to this transformation for the SSD user. Finally, we discuss ways of closing this “modality gap” found in SSDs and conclude that this process is best benchmarked against performance with SSDs that return to their primary modality (e.g., visuospatial into visual).

[1]  J. Martinovic,et al.  An Advantage for Active versus Passive Aperture-Viewing in Visual Object Recognition , 2011, Perception.

[2]  D R Perrott,et al.  Minimum audible angle thresholds for sources varying in both elevation and azimuth. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Marc O Ernst,et al.  Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Multi-aperture viewing: perception of figures through very small apertures , 1993, Vision Research.

[5]  Tina Iachini,et al.  Egocentric/allocentric and coordinate/categorical haptic encoding in blind people , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[6]  Melvyn A Goodale,et al.  The Effects of Different Aperture-Viewing Conditions on the Recognition of Novel Objects , 2003, Perception.

[7]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  Differentiated audio-tactile correspondences in sighted and blind individuals. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Representing colour through hearing and touch in sensory substitution devices. , 2013, Multisensory research.

[9]  G. Sandini,et al.  Impairment of auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human subjects , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  Roni Y. Granot,et al.  Listening in the dark: Congenital and early blindness and cross-domain mappings in music. , 2012 .

[11]  Beatriz Defez,et al.  Sensory navigation device for blind people , 2013 .

[12]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Recruitment of Occipital Cortex during Sensory Substitution Training Linked to Subjective Experience of Seeing in People with Blindness , 2011, PloS one.

[13]  T. Donaldson Robustness of the F-Test to Errors of Both Kinds and the Correlation Between the Numerator and Denominator of the F-Ratio , 1968 .

[14]  Árni Kristjánsson,et al.  Relative vibrotactile spatial acuity of the torso , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  J. Loomis On the tangibility of letters and braille , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  M. P. Zwiers,et al.  A Spatial Hearing Deficit in Early-Blind Humans , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Laurent Renier,et al.  Vertical-horizontal illusion present for sighted but not early blind humans using auditory substitution of vision , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Ingrid M. Kanics,et al.  Tactile Acuity is Enhanced in Blindness , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  S Weinstein,et al.  Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality , 1968 .

[20]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Development of a tactile vest , 2004, 12th International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 2004. HAPTICS '04. Proceedings..

[21]  J. Ward,et al.  Sound Properties Associated With Equiluminant Colours. , 2017, Multisensory research.

[22]  Árni Kristjánsson,et al.  The Sound of Vision Project: On the Feasibility of an Audio-Haptic Representation of the Environment, for the Visually Impaired , 2016, Brain sciences.

[23]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is used in maintenance of spatial orientation , 2008, Cognition.

[24]  Olivier Collignon,et al.  Is there a future for sensory substitution outside academic laboratories? , 2014, Multisensory research.

[25]  M. Proulx,et al.  Visual experience facilitates allocentric spatial representation , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  Yoshitaka Nakajima,et al.  Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound Albert S. Bregman , 1992 .

[27]  R Bruyer,et al.  The Ponzo Illusion with Auditory Substitution of Vision in Sighted and Early-Blind Subjects , 2005, Perception.

[28]  F. Rösler,et al.  Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[30]  Denis Pellerin,et al.  Depth — Melody substitution , 2012, 2012 Proceedings of the 20th European Signal Processing Conference (EUSIPCO).

[31]  Monica Gori,et al.  Auditory and proprioceptive spatial impairments in blind children and adults. , 2017, Developmental science.

[32]  J. Ward,et al.  The role of visual experience in the emergence of cross-modal correspondences , 2018, Cognition.

[33]  Michael J. Proulx,et al.  How well do you see what you hear? The acuity of visual-to-auditory sensory substitution , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[34]  P Bartolomeo,et al.  Brain networks of spatial awareness: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging tractography , 2007, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[35]  Paul Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Brain plasticity: ‘visual’ acuity of blind persons via the tongue , 2001, Brain Research.

[36]  Ómar I. Jóhannesson,et al.  Designing sensory-substitution devices: Principles, pitfalls and potential , 2016, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[37]  Andrew J. R. Simpson,et al.  Visual objects in the auditory system in sensory substitution: how much information do we need? , 2014, Multisensory research.

[38]  R. Cholewiak,et al.  Vibrotactile localization on the abdomen: Effects of place and space , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Peter B. L. Meijer,et al.  Visual experiences in the blind induced by an auditory sensory substitution device , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Developing Spatial Knowledge in the Absence of Vision: Allocentric and Egocentric Representations Generated by Blind People When Supported by Auditory Cues , 2010 .

[41]  Andrew J. R. Simpson,et al.  Auditory scene analysis and sonified visual images. Does consonance negatively impact on object formation when using complex sonified stimuli? , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[42]  J M Loomis,et al.  Counterexample to the hypothesis of functional similarity between tactile and visual pattern perception , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  G. Baud-Bovy,et al.  Multisensory Rehabilitation Training Improves Spatial Perception in Totally but Not Partially Visually Deprived Children , 2017, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[44]  Jennifer Lisa Rochlis,et al.  A vibrotactile display for aiding extravehicular activity (EVA) navigation in space , 1998 .

[45]  David C. Reutens,et al.  Early but not late-blindness leads to enhanced auditory perception , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Denis Pellerin,et al.  Navigating from a Depth Image Converted into Sound , 2015, Applied bionics and biomechanics.

[47]  J.L. Gonzalez-Mora,et al.  Seeing the world by hearing: Virtual Acoustic Space (VAS) a new space perception system for blind people. , 2006, 2006 2nd International Conference on Information & Communication Technologies.

[48]  Jan O. Borchers,et al.  VibroVision: An On-Body Tactile Image Guide for the Blind , 2016, CHI Extended Abstracts.

[49]  Tina Iachini,et al.  Does blindness affect egocentric and allocentric frames of reference in small and large scale spaces? , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[50]  J. Norman,et al.  Blindness enhances tactile acuity and haptic 3-D shape discrimination , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[51]  G. Glass,et al.  Consequences of Failure to Meet Assumptions Underlying the Fixed Effects Analyses of Variance and Covariance , 1972 .

[52]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Auditory Spatial Tuning in Late-onset Blindness in Humans , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  Jörg Lewald,et al.  Vertical sound localization in blind humans , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Christoph M. Friedrich,et al.  Object Detection Featuring 3D Audio Localization for Microsoft HoloLens - A Deep Learning based Sensor Substitution Approach for the Blind , 2018, HEALTHINF.

[55]  Brian C. J. Moore,et al.  Auditory spatial representations of the world are compressed in blind humans , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  V R Algazi,et al.  Elevation localization and head-related transfer function analysis at low frequencies. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[57]  Andy P. Field,et al.  Discovering Statistics Using Ibm Spss Statistics , 2017 .

[58]  J. Tukey,et al.  Performance of Some Resistant Rules for Outlier Labeling , 1986 .

[59]  Alex Pentland,et al.  A wearable haptic navigation guidance system , 1998, Digest of Papers. Second International Symposium on Wearable Computers (Cat. No.98EX215).

[60]  Amy C Nau,et al.  Visual task performance in the blind with the BrainPort V100 Vision Aid , 2016, Expert Review of Medical Devices.

[61]  J M Findlay,et al.  Aperture Viewing*: A Review and a Synthesis , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[62]  Tina Iachini,et al.  Congenital blindness limits allocentric to egocentric switching ability , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  Michael J. Proulx,et al.  The role of visual experience for the neural basis of spatial cognition , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[64]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Hearing Cheats Touch, but Less in Congenitally Blind Than in Sighted Individuals , 2004, Psychological science.

[65]  K. Horch,et al.  Mobility performance with a pixelized vision system , 1992, Vision Research.

[66]  David J. Brown,et al.  Audio–Vision Substitution for Blind Individuals: Addressing Human Information Processing Capacity Limitations , 2016, IEEE Journal of Selected Topics in Signal Processing.

[67]  C. Avendano,et al.  The CIPIC HRTF database , 2001, Proceedings of the 2001 IEEE Workshop on the Applications of Signal Processing to Audio and Acoustics (Cat. No.01TH8575).

[68]  H. Colburn,et al.  Sensitivity of human subjects to head-related transfer-function phase spectra. , 1999, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[69]  Jan B. F. van Erp,et al.  Vibrotactile Spatial Acuity on the Torso: Effects of Location and Timing Parameters , 2005, WHC.

[70]  Monica Gori,et al.  Shape Perception and Navigation in Blind Adults , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[71]  José L. González-Mora,et al.  Development of a New Space Perception System for Blind People, Based on the Creation of a Virtual Acoustic Space , 1999, IWANN.

[72]  Mel Slater,et al.  A first person avatar system with haptic feedback , 2010, VRST '10.

[73]  Andrew J. Kolarik,et al.  Evidence for enhanced discrimination of virtual auditory distance among blind listeners using level and direct-to-reverberant cues , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  F. Wightman,et al.  A model of head-related transfer functions based on principal components analysis and minimum-phase reconstruction. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[75]  J C Stevens,et al.  Spatial acuity of the body surface over the life span. , 1996, Somatosensory & motor research.

[76]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans , 1999, Nature.

[77]  Stefano Baldan,et al.  Auditory depth map representations with a sensory substitution scheme based on synthetic fluid sounds , 2017, 2017 IEEE 19th International Workshop on Multimedia Signal Processing (MMSP).

[78]  A. Materka,et al.  Individual HRTF measurements for accurate obstacle sonification in an electronic travel aid for the blind , 2008, 2008 International Conference on Signals and Electronic Systems.

[79]  N. Giudice Navigating without vision: principles of blind spatial cognition , 2018 .

[80]  Peter B. L. Meijer,et al.  An experimental system for auditory image representations , 1992, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[81]  Thomas W. James,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration of face, object, and scene features in occipito-temporal cortex , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[82]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Learning to Perceive with a Visuo — Auditory Substitution System: Localisation and Object Recognition with ‘The Voice’ , 2007, Perception.

[83]  J M Loomis,et al.  Analysis of tactile and visual confusion matrices , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[84]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[85]  Marianna Obrist,et al.  "I Always Wanted to See the Night Sky": Blind User Preferences for Sensory Substitution Devices , 2016, CHI.

[86]  François Michel,et al.  Seeing without the Occipito-Parietal Cortex: Simultagnosia as a Shrinkage of the Attentional Visual Field , 2004, Behavioural neurology.

[87]  Achille Pasqualotto,et al.  Sensory Substitution: The Spatial Updating of Auditory Scenes “Mimics” the Spatial Updating of Visual Scenes , 2016, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[88]  Tilen Potisk,et al.  Head-Related Transfer Function , 2015 .

[89]  T. Cornsweet,et al.  The staircrase-method in psychophysics. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[90]  J. Blauert Spatial Hearing: The Psychophysics of Human Sound Localization , 1983 .

[91]  M. Gori,et al.  Early visual deprivation prompts the use of body-centered frames of reference for auditory localization , 2018, Cognition.

[92]  K O Johnson,et al.  A comparison of visual and two modes of tactual letter resolution , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[93]  Amir Amedi,et al.  ‘Visual’ Acuity of the Congenitally Blind Using Visual-to-Auditory Sensory Substitution , 2012, PloS one.

[94]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Cross-Modal Correspondences Enhance Performance on a Colour-to-Sound Sensory Substitution Device. , 2016, Multisensory research.

[95]  J M Kennedy,et al.  Perspective taking, pictures, and the blind , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[96]  Shachar Maidenbaum,et al.  Author's Personal Copy Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Sensory Substitution: Closing the Gap between Basic Research and Widespread Practical Visual Rehabilitation Author's Personal Copy , 2022 .

[97]  BENJAMIN WHITE,et al.  Vision Substitution by Tactile Image Projection , 1969, Nature.

[98]  Adrian Burlacu,et al.  Computer Vision for the Visually Impaired: the Sound of Vision System , 2017, 2017 IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision Workshops (ICCVW).

[99]  H. Christiaan Stronks,et al.  Training Improves Vibrotactile Spatial Acuity and Intensity Discrimination on the Lower Back Using Coin Motors , 2017, Artificial organs.

[100]  Miguel A. Torres,et al.  Sensory Substitution for Visually Disabled People: Computer Solutions , 2010 .

[101]  Thomas D. Wright,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Sensory Substitution as an Artificially Acquired Synaesthesia , 2022 .

[102]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Haptic perception: A tutorial , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[103]  Christopher Fisher,et al.  Clinical Tests of Ultra-Low Vision Used to Evaluate Rudimentary Visual Perceptions Enabled by the BrainPort Vision Device. , 2013, Translational vision science & technology.

[104]  Monica Gori,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2022 .

[105]  Malika Auvray,et al.  The process of distal attribution illuminated through studies of sensory substitution. , 2014, Multisensory research.

[106]  R. Day,et al.  Illusions of time and extent when the Müller-Lyer figure moves in an aperture , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[107]  T. Stafford,et al.  Improving training for sensory augmentation using the science of expertise , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[108]  Fernando Bermejo,et al.  Sensorimotor strategies for recognizing geometrical shapes: a comparative study with different sensory substitution devices , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[109]  D. Perrott,et al.  Minimum Audible Movement Angle as a Function of the Azimuth and Elevation of the Source , 1992, Human factors.

[110]  J. Craig,et al.  Vision is superior to touch in shape perception even with equivalent peripheral input. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.