The Dissociation between Early and Late Selection in Older Adults
暂无分享,去创建一个
Phillip J. Holcomb | Kirk R. Daffner | Anna E. Haring | Brittany R. Alperin | Tatyana Y. Zhuravleva | Dorene M. Rentz | P. Holcomb | D. Rentz | K. Daffner | T. Zhuravleva | A. E. Haring | B. Alperin
[1] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[2] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Age-related differences in performance and stimulus processing in dual task situation , 2011, Brain Research.
[3] R. West,et al. Visual distraction, working memory, and aging , 1999, Memory & cognition.
[4] D Friedman,et al. An overview of age-related changes in the scalp distribution of P3b. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[5] P. Ghisletta,et al. A dynamic investigation of cognitive dedifferentiation with control for retest: evidence from the Swiss Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study on the Oldest Old. , 2005, Psychology and aging.
[6] S. Folstein,et al. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.
[7] Brittany R. Alperin,et al. Age-related decline in differentiated neural responses to rare target versus frequent standard stimuli , 2014, Brain Research.
[8] J. Yesavage,et al. Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] P. Baltes,et al. Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? , 1997, Psychology and aging.
[10] G. Smith,et al. Slowness and age: speed-accuracy mechanisms. , 1995, Psychology and aging.
[11] D. Tucker,et al. Frontal evaluation and posterior representation in target detection. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[12] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[13] G. Brébion,et al. Language Processing, Slowing, and Speed/Accuracy Trade-Off in the Elderly , 2001, Experimental aging research.
[14] Albert Kok,et al. Cerebral event-related potentials associated with selective attention to color: Developmental changes from childhood to adulthood , 1998 .
[15] Denise C. Park,et al. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.
[16] E. Walker,et al. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .
[17] Maro G. Machizawa,et al. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.
[18] H. Semlitsch,et al. Multichannel auditory event-related brain potentials: effects of normal aging on the scalp distribution of N1, P2, N2 and P300 latencies and amplitudes. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[19] Hauke R. Heekeren,et al. Normal Aging Delays and Compromises Early Multifocal Visual Attention during Object Tracking , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[21] Zara M. Bergström,et al. Behavioral and ERP evidence of greater distractor processing in old age , 2009, Brain Research.
[22] Theodore P. Zanto,et al. Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] Adam Gazzaley,et al. Early Top–Down Control of Visual Processing Predicts Working Memory Performance , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Brittany R. Alperin,et al. Investigating the age-related "anterior shift" in the scalp distribution of the P3b component using principal component analysis. , 2014, Psychophysiology.
[25] Glenn F. Wilson,et al. Task decision difficulty: effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task , 1994, Biological Psychology.
[26] E. Wagenmakers,et al. The Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in the Elderly Brain: A Structural Model-Based Approach , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] A Kok,et al. Selective processing of two-dimensional visual stimuli in young and old subjects: electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[28] Thomas F Münte,et al. Temporal dynamics of early perceptual processing , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[29] Bruno Kopp,et al. Temporal dynamics of selective attention and conflict resolution during cross-dimensional go-nogo decisions , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.
[30] P. Rabbitt. How old and young subjects monitor and control responses for accuracy and speed , 1979 .
[31] Adam Gazzaley,et al. Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults , 2010, Cortex.
[32] Andreas Keil,et al. Neuronal Synchronization and Selective Color Processing in the Human Brain , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[33] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to color and location: An analysis with event-related brain potentials , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[35] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Compensatory neural activity distinguishes different patterns of normal cognitive aging , 2008, NeuroImage.
[36] T. Salthouse. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.
[37] David L. Strayer,et al. Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. , 1994, Psychology and aging.
[38] Joseph J. Ryan,et al. A screening procedure for estimating premorbid intelligence in the elderly , 1992 .
[39] Hyemi Chong,et al. Cognitive status impacts age-related changes in attention to novel and target events in normal adults. , 2007, Neuropsychology.
[40] V. Leirer,et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. , 1982, Journal of psychiatric research.
[41] Andreas Keil,et al. Selective Processing of Multiple Features in the Human Brain: Effects of Feature Type and Salience , 2011, PloS one.
[42] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. The influence of executive capacity on selective attention and subsequent processing , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[43] D. Filion,et al. Aging, selective attention, and inhibitory processes: a psychophysiological approach. , 1992, Psychology and Aging.
[44] T. Curran,et al. Effects of aging on visuospatial attention: an ERP study , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[45] Axel Mecklinger,et al. Age differences in attentional control: an event-related potential approach. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[46] I Czigler,et al. Age, color processing and meaningfulness: an event-related potential study. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[47] Joaquin A. Anguera,et al. Dissociation of motor and sensory inhibition processes in normal aging , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[48] R. Knight,et al. Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[49] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Does compensatory neural activity survive old-old age? , 2011, NeuroImage.
[50] Kirk R. Daffner,et al. Age-related changes in early novelty processing as measured by ERPs , 2009, Biological Psychology.
[51] A. Kok,et al. Early and late selection in young and old adults: an event-related potential study. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[52] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Mechanisms Underlying Age- and Performance-related Differences in Working Memory , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[53] Lynn Hasher,et al. Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .
[54] David A. Bennett,et al. Neural intrinsic connectivity networks associated with risk aversion in old age , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.
[55] J. Botwinick. Aging and Behavior , 1984, Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
[56] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[57] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[58] Siu Cheung Li,et al. Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotrans , 1999 .
[59] M. Fabiani,et al. Individual differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and their relationship to frontal lobe function. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[60] Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[61] E. Kaplan,et al. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System , 2012 .
[62] Kristine B Walhovd,et al. Life-span changes in P3a. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[63] R. West,et al. The Effects of Aging on Controlled Attention and Conflict Processing in the Stroop Task , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[64] Anna E. Haring,et al. Age-related differences in enhancement and suppression of neural activity underlying selective attention in matched young and old adults , 2013, Brain Research.
[65] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials , 2008, Brain Research.
[66] D. Wechsler. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale , 2021, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.
[67] Risa Sawaki,et al. Stimulus context determines whether non-target stimuli are processed as task-relevant or distractor information , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[68] S. Luck,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.
[69] T. Toulopoulou,et al. Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. , 2008, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[70] Dc Washington. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .
[71] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Increased Responsiveness to Novelty is Associated with Successful Cognitive Aging , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[72] E Donchin,et al. Resource reciprocity: an event-related brain potentials analysis. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[73] M. D’Esposito,et al. The effect of non-visual working memory load on top-down modulation of visual processing , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[74] E. Tangalos,et al. Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO , 1996 .
[75] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. Does modulation of selective attention to features reflect enhancement or suppression of neural activity? , 2012, Biological Psychology.
[76] C. Frith,et al. The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.
[77] A Kok,et al. Cerebral event-related potentials associated with selective attention to color: developmental changes from childhood to adulthood. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[78] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Dedifferentiation in the visual cortex: An fMRI investigation of individual differences in older adults , 2008, Brain Research.
[79] C Alain,et al. Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[80] M. Schlossberg. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. , 1986 .
[81] N. Cohen,et al. Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fMRI Study of the Stroop Task , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[82] J. J. Ryan,et al. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .
[83] E. Donchin,et al. Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.
[84] Matthew W. Miller,et al. A novel approach to the physiological measurement of mental workload. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[85] Axel Mecklinger,et al. Age differences in task switching and response monitoring: Evidence from ERPs , 2007, Biological Psychology.
[86] Risa Sawaki,et al. Distractor P3 is associated with attentional capture by stimulus deviance , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[87] Shulan Hsieh,et al. Elderly adults through compensatory responses can be just as capable as young adults in inhibiting the flanker influence , 2012, Biological Psychology.
[88] H. Levin. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1993 .
[89] R. Cabeza,et al. Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[90] R. Cabeza,et al. Age-related changes in hemispheric organization , 2005 .
[91] R. K. Simpson. Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .
[92] O. Spreen,et al. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .
[93] Risto N t nen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[94] J. Botwinick,et al. Cautiousness in advanced age. , 1966, Journal of gerontology.
[95] G Mulder,et al. Attention to color: an analysis of selection, controlled search, and motor activation, using event-related potentials. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[96] T. Robbins,et al. Risk taking during decision-making in normal volunteers changes with age , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[97] Lynn Hasher,et al. Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a "new view" , 2007 .
[98] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Selective attention to spatial and non-spatial visual stimuli is affected differentially by age: effects on event-related brain potentials and performance data. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[99] L. Lorenzo-López,et al. Visual target processing in high- and low-performing older subjects indexed by P3 component , 2007, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.