Behavioral and physiological measures in the detection of concealed information.

The authors examined the incremental validity of the reaction time (RT) measure beyond that of skin conductance response (SCR) in the detection of concealed information. Participants performed a Stroop-like task in which they named the color of critical and neutral words. Results show that the SCR highly differentiated between the relevant and neutral words. However, the RT demonstrated a significant differentiation only when the critical words denoted personally significant items (e.g., one's own name) and not when they denoted crime-relevant items related to a simulated crime. In both cases, combining the 2 measures yielded no advantage over the use of SCR alone. Thus, although behavioral measures may differentiate between relevant and neutral information in some cases, their practical use is questionable.

[1]  J. Allen,et al.  The identification of concealed memories using the event-related potential and implicit behavioral measures: a methodology for prediction in the face of individual differences. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[2]  H Pashler,et al.  Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: distinct attentional mechanisms. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  M. Dawson,et al.  Is elicitation of the autonomic orienting response associated with allocation of processing resources? , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[4]  D. Siddle,et al.  Orienting, habituation, and resource allocation: an associative analysis. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[5]  D. Siddle,et al.  Stimulus omission and dishabituation of the electrodermal orienting response: the allocation of processing resources. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[6]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Orienting to guilty knowledge , 2004, Cognition & emotion.

[7]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[8]  D. Dougherty,et al.  The validity of polygraph testing: Scientific analysis and public controversy. , 1985 .

[9]  E Donchin,et al.  The truth will out: interrogative polygraphy ("lie detection") with event-related brain potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[10]  D. Siddle,et al.  Effects of intermodality change on electrodermal orienting and on the allocation of processing resources. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Theories and Applications in the Detection of Deception: A Psychophysiological and International Perspective , 1989 .

[12]  Electrodermal differentiation of deception: potentially confounding and influencing factors. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  D. Siddle,et al.  Stimulus omission and dishabituation of the skin conductance response. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[14]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Modern scientific evidence : the law and science of expert testimony , 2006 .

[15]  Takehiko Yamamura,et al.  Development of the polygraph technique in Japan for detection of deception , 1990 .

[16]  David C. Raskin,et al.  Polygraph techniques for the detection of deception. , 1989 .

[17]  D. Bamber The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph , 1975 .

[18]  David T. Lykken,et al.  Why (some) Americans believe in the lie detector while others believe in the guilty knowledge test , 1991, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[19]  M. Dawson,et al.  Probing the orienting response with startle modification and secondary reaction time. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[20]  J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Simple, effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[21]  O. Lipp,et al.  Effects of intermodality change and number of training trials on electrodermal orienting and on the allocation of processing resources , 1996, Biological Psychology.

[22]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Novelty and significance in orientation and habituation: a feature-matching approach. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Standardization within individuals: a simple method to neutralize individual differences in skin conductance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[24]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: a meta-analytic review. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[25]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Attention and the Processing of Emotional Words and Names , 2004, Psychological science.

[26]  G. S. Klein,et al.  SEMANTIC POWER MEASURED THROUGH THE INTERFERENCE OF WORDS WITH COLOR-NAMING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[27]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  of the Control Questions Test ( CQT ) , 2003 .

[28]  D. Lykken,et al.  Psychology and the lie detector industry. , 1974, The American psychologist.

[29]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Effects of questions' repetition and variation on the efficiency of the guilty knowledge test: a reexamination. , 2002, The Journal of applied psychology.

[30]  Fred E. Inbau,et al.  Truth and deception : the polygraph (lie-detector) technique , 1977 .

[31]  Charles R. Honts,et al.  The Scientific Status of Research on Polygraph Techniques: The Case for Polygraph Tests , 2002 .

[32]  J. Allen,et al.  A comparison of methods for the analysis of event-related potentials in deception detection. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[33]  C. Honts,et al.  Mental and physical countermeasures reduce the accuracy of the concealed knowledge test. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[34]  I. Lieblich,et al.  Guilty knowledge technique: application of signal detection measures. , 1970, The Journal of applied psychology.

[35]  V. Maclaren,et al.  A quantitative review of the guilty knowledge test. , 2001, The Journal of applied psychology.

[36]  I. Wilkinson,et al.  Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.

[37]  C M Seifert,et al.  Using response time measures to assess "guilty knowledge". , 2000, The Journal of applied psychology.

[38]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[39]  D. C. Raskin,et al.  Physiological measures and the detection of deception. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Leakage of relevant information to innocent examinees in the GKT : An attempt to reduce false-positive outcomes by introducing target stimuli , 1999 .

[41]  Murray Kleiner,et al.  Handbook of polygraph testing. , 2002 .

[42]  E. N. Solokov Perception and the conditioned reflex , 1963 .

[43]  David C. Raskin,et al.  Psychological methods in criminal investigation and evidence , 1989 .

[44]  Ioannis Pavlidis,et al.  Seeing through the face of deception , 2002, Nature.

[45]  H. Timm Effect of altered outcome expectancies stemming from placebo and feedback treatments on the validity of the guilty knowledge technique. , 1982 .

[46]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[47]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Psychophysiological detection through the guilty knowledge technique: effects of mental countermeasures. , 1996, The Journal of applied psychology.

[48]  I. Maltzman,et al.  Orienting reflexes and significance: a reply to O'Gorman. , 1979, Psychophysiology.

[49]  J P Rosenfeld,et al.  A modified, event-related potential-based guilty knowledge test. , 1988, The International journal of neuroscience.

[50]  Gary E. Raney,et al.  Detection of guilty knowledge with event-related potentials. , 1991 .

[51]  R J Cutrow,et al.  The objective use of multiple physiological indices in the detection of deception. , 1972, Psychophysiology.

[52]  E. Hazlett,et al.  The relationship between skin conductance orienting and the allocation of processing resources. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[53]  S. Messick Validity of Psychological Assessment: Validation of Inferences from Persons' Responses and Performances as Scientific Inquiry into Score Meaning. Research Report RR-94-45. , 1994 .

[54]  Bruno Verschuere,et al.  Autonomic and behavioral responding to concealed information: differentiating orienting and defensive responses. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[55]  David T. Lykken,et al.  The validity of the guilty knowledge technique: The effects of faking. , 1960 .

[56]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  H. Timm Effect of biofeedback on the detection of deception. , 1987, Journal of forensic sciences.

[58]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Admissibility of polygraph tests: The application of scientific standards post- Daubert . , 1999 .

[59]  Chetwyn C. H. Chan,et al.  Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[60]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Dissociations of personally significant and task-relevant distractors inside and outside the focus of attention: a combined behavioral and psychophysiological study. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[61]  A. Leontyev,et al.  Psychological research in the U.S.S.R , 1966 .

[62]  Gershon Ben-Shakhar,et al.  The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) as an application of psychophysiology: Future prospects and obstacles. , 2002 .

[63]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Effects of item repetitions and variations on the efficiency of the guilty knowledge test. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[64]  A. S. Bernstein,et al.  The orienting response as novelty and significance detector: reply to O'Gorman. , 1979, Psychophysiology.

[65]  David T. Lykken,et al.  A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector , 1980 .

[66]  David M. Green,et al.  General Prediction Relating Yes‐No and Forced‐Choice Results , 1964 .

[67]  Stephen J. Boies,et al.  Components of attention. , 1971 .