Foveal fixation and tracking in the praying mantis

Summary1.Visually guided head and body movements of restrained and freely moving mantids (Tenodera australasiae) have been studied by means of closed circuit television. Interest was concentrated on the different roles of the fovea and the periphery of the eye in controlling visuo-motor behaviour.2.The peripheral eye is mainly responsible for the detection of novel objects (preferably potential prey) and the generation of ballistic (open-loop) saccadic head movements (Fig. 2) which bring the target image to the fovea (Figs. 3, 4, 5, 6).3.Measurements on monocular animals show that the fovea ofeach eye is encircled by a saccade sensitive periphery (Figs. 5, 6). In other words each eye is capable of measuring any retinal position of the target image in a coordinate system whose origin is at the fovea. Based on this finding, a hypothesis, which is outlined in Sect. IV.7, suggests that the binocular coordination during fixation, tracking and distance estimation is based on the comparison of angular coordinates extracted by each eye from the position vector of the target.4.Moving targets which have been fixated are held in the fovea either by smooth or saccadic tracking eye movements. The degree to which either tracking strategy is employed depends mainly on the features of the background, but to some extent also on the velocity of the target.5.Targets which move against a homogeneous background are tracked by smooth eye movements (Fig. 7). Low target angular velocities are closely matched by the eye velocity. At high target speeds the head lags increasingly behind the target and saccades are periodically required to reduce the position error relative to the fovea.6.Smooth pursuit eye movements, evoked either by a single target (Fig. 7) or a disrupted background (Fig. 8), are affected primarily by the velocity of the retinal image. While the effects of target and background are similar in this respect, they differ in others. Small objects in the foreground, subtending an angle of only a few degrees on the retina, evoke strong pursuit responses only when they resemble typical prey and project onto the fovea (Fig. 9). On the other hand, the image stabilisation of the background is a stereotyped response that can be evoked whenever a large part of the background moves across the visual field (Fig. 10a). Moreover, responses caused by a moving target in the fovea, and movements of the background in the periphery, are not combined additively (Fig. 10b). The foveal tracking response is weighted more strongly, but because the target is usually small, compared with the background, competing background motion can suppress smooth foveal tracking almost completely.7.This limitation imposed upon the smooth pursuit system by the presence of a disrupted background (either a stripe pattern in the experimental set-up or grass and other plants in a natural setting) is avoided by the adoption of a strategy of saccadic tracking (Figs. 11, 12, 15, 16). This also applies for the tracking which immediately precedes the catching of prey (Fig. 15). Therefore, the stabilisation of the target image in place on the fovea is not a prerequisite for a successful strike.8.Up to target angular velocities of about 100 °/s, saccadic tracking is predictive, i.e. the saccades have adequate amplitudes to bring the fovea right on target at the instant the saccade is completed (Fig. 13). This implies that the saccadic system processes not only position information of the target but velocity information as well. It is suggested that this velocity information is provided by the smooth pursuit system. Saccadic tracking would then reflect interactions of two circuits, the velocity coding circuit which in the presence of a homogeneous background also generates smooth pursuit head movements, and the position coding circuit which in the absence of target movement is able to generate saccades on its own.

[1]  C. W. Oyster,et al.  Direction-selective retinal ganglion cells and control of optokinetic nystagmus in the rabbit. , 1972, Vision research.

[2]  C. Rowell,et al.  Saccadic suppression by corollary discharge in the locust , 1979, Nature.

[3]  W. Buddenbrock,et al.  DIE PHYSIOLOGIE DES FACETTENAUGES , 1935 .

[4]  H. Maldonado,et al.  A fovea in the praying mantis eye , 1970, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[5]  Heinrich F. Friederichs,et al.  Beiträge zur morphologie und physiologie der sehorgane der cicindelinen (Col.) , 1931, Zeitschrift für Morphologie und Ökologie der Tiere.

[6]  H. Mittelstaedt Prey capture in Mantids , 1957 .

[7]  Thomas S. Collett,et al.  SHORT COMMUNICATION PEERING - A LOCUST BEHAVIOUR PATTERN FOR OBTAINING MOTION PARALLAX INFORMATION , 1978 .

[8]  V E Pichka [Pathways for the transmission of visual information in the protocerebrum of the drone fly Eristalis tenax]. , 1976, Zhurnal evoliutsionnoi biokhimii i fiziologii.

[9]  H Maldonado,et al.  Depth perception in the praying mantis. , 1972, Physiology & behavior.

[10]  H. Maldonado,et al.  Distance estimation and the monocular cleaning reflex in praying mantis , 1967, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[11]  M F Land,et al.  Movements of the retinae of jumping spiders (Salticidae: dendryphantinae) in response to visual stimuli. , 1969, The Journal of experimental biology.

[12]  C. G. Mueller,et al.  Saccadic head movements in mantids , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[13]  P. Duelli,et al.  Movement detection in the posterolateral eyes of jumping spiders (Evarcha arcuata, Salticidae) , 1978, Journal of comparative physiology.

[14]  D C Sandeman,et al.  A sensitive position measuring device for biological systems. , 1968, Comparative biochemistry and physiology.

[15]  Giulio Fermi,et al.  Optomotorische Reaktionen der Fliege Musca Domestica , 1963, Kybernetik.

[16]  W Reichardt,et al.  Visual control of orientation behaviour in the fly: Part II. Towards the underlying neural interactions , 1976, Quarterly Reviews of Biophysics.

[17]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Prey recognition in the praying mantis , 1959 .

[18]  L. Stark THE CONTROL SYSTEM FOR VERSIONAL EYE MOVEMENTS , 1971 .

[19]  Nicolas Franceschini,et al.  Sampling of the Visual Environment by the Compound Eye of the Fly: Fundamentals and Applications , 1975 .

[20]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[21]  Karl Baldus,et al.  Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die Entfernungslokalisation der Libellen (Aeschna Cyanea) , 1926, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[22]  D. Burkhardt,et al.  Zum binokularen Entfernungssehen der Insekten , 1973, Journal of comparative physiology.

[23]  W Reichardt,et al.  Visual control of orientation behaviour in the fly: Part I. A quantitative analysis , 1976, Quarterly Reviews of Biophysics.

[24]  T. Collett,et al.  Chasing behaviour of houseflies (Fannia canicularis) , 1974, Journal of comparative physiology.

[25]  W. Becker The control of eye movements in the saccadic system. , 1972, Bibliotheca ophthalmologica : supplementa ad ophthalmologica.

[26]  J Kien,et al.  Sensory integration in the locust optomotor system--I: behavioural analysis. , 1974, Vision research.

[27]  N H Barmack,et al.  Modification of eye movements by instantaneous changes in the velocity of visual targets. , 1970, Vision research.

[28]  G Westheimer,et al.  Virtually guided movements: Group report , 1977 .

[29]  M. F. LAND,et al.  Head Movement of Flies during Visually Guided Flight , 1973, Nature.

[30]  Samuel Rossel,et al.  Regional differences in photoreceptor performance in the eye of the praying mantis , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.

[31]  R. W. Ditchburn Eye-movements and visual perception , 1973 .

[32]  M. Land Orientation by jumping spiders in the absence of visual feedback. , 1971, The Journal of experimental biology.

[33]  W. Reichardt,et al.  Detection and tracking of moving objects by the fly Musca domestica , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  T. Collett,et al.  How hoverflies compute interception courses , 1978, Journal of comparative physiology.

[35]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Firing patterns of abducens neurons of alert monkeys in relationship to horizontal eye movement. , 1970, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Albert F. Fuchs,et al.  THE SACCADIC SYSTEM , 1971 .

[37]  J. Erber,et al.  Optokinetic eye movements in the crab,Carcinus maenas , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[38]  T. Collett,et al.  Visual control of flight behaviour in the hoverflySyritta pipiens L. , 1975, Journal of comparative physiology.

[39]  G. Horridge,et al.  Nervous control of optokinetic responses in the crab Carcinus , 1964, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[40]  H. Maldonado,et al.  Study of the role of the binocular vision in mantids to estimate long distances, using the deimatic reaction as experimental situation , 1970, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[41]  David A. Robinson,et al.  Models of the saccadic eye movement control system , 1973, Kybernetik.

[42]  H. Collewijn Optokinetic eye movements in the rabbit: input-output relations. , 1969, Vision research.

[43]  Bernward Pick,et al.  Visual Flicker Induces Orientation Behaviour in the Fly Musca , 1974 .

[44]  R. Wolf,et al.  On the fine structure of yaw torque in visual flight orientation ofDrosophila melanogaster , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.

[45]  J Kien,et al.  Sensory integration in the locust optomotor system--II: direction selective neurons in the circumoesophageal connectives and the optic lobe. , 1974, Vision research.

[46]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[47]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[48]  Gilbert D. McCann,et al.  Binocular interactions of motion detection fibers in the optic lobes of flies , 1971, Kybernetik.

[49]  P. H. Schiller The discharge characteristics of single units in the oculomotor and abducens nuclei of the unanesthetized monkey , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  George Adrian Horridge,et al.  Anatomy of the Regional Differences in the Eye of the Mantis Ciulfina , 1979 .

[51]  G. K. Wallace Visual Scanning in the Desert Locust Schistocerca Gregaria Forskål , 1959 .