Development of socio-emotional competence in bonobos

Significance Across human development, individuals better able to manage their own emotions show greater social competence and more empathic concern for others. To test this interplay between social and emotional skills in one of our closest relatives, we collected behavioral measures on bonobos (Pan paniscus) with different rearing backgrounds at a forested sanctuary in Africa. Young bonobos showed the same connection between the ability to regulate their own emotions and social competence, such as developing friendships and concern for others. Mother-reared juveniles performed far better in this regard than juveniles orphaned at a young age, thus highlighting the importance of the mother–offspring bond. Our results support a shared socio-emotional framework for human and nonhuman primate behavior. Social and emotional skills are tightly interlinked in human development, and both are negatively impacted by disrupted social development. The same interplay between social and emotional skills, including expressions of empathy, has received scant attention in other primates however, despite the growing interest in caring, friendships, and the fitness benefits of social skills. Here we examine the development of socio-emotional competence in juvenile bonobos (Pan paniscus) at a sanctuary in the Democratic Republic of the Congo, focusing on the interplay between various skills, including empathy-related responding. Most subjects were rehabilitated orphans, but some were born at the sanctuary and mother-reared there. We observed how juveniles with different rearing backgrounds responded to stressful events, both when the stress affected themselves (e.g., a lost fight) or others (e.g., witnessing the distress of others). The main dependent variable was the consolation of distressed parties by means of calming body contact. As in children, consolation was predicted by overall social competence and effective emotion regulation, as reflected in the speed of recovery from self-distress and behavioral measures of anxiety. Juveniles more effective at self-regulation were more likely to console others in distress, and such behavior was more typical of mother-reared juveniles than orphans. These results highlight the interplay between the development of social and emotional skills in our ape relatives and the importance of the mother–offspring bond in shaping socio-emotional competence.

[1]  E. Palagi,et al.  Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus) , 2004, American journal of primatology.

[2]  D. Rumbaugh,et al.  Long-Term Cognitive Deficits in Chimpanzees Associated with Early Impoverished Rearing. , 1973 .

[3]  K. Stafford,et al.  Eye temperature and heart rate variability of calves disbudded with or without local anaesthetic , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.

[4]  B. Quinn,et al.  Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. , 2010, Developmental science.

[5]  L. Parr Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[6]  J. D. Welsh,et al.  Psychophysiological correlates of empathy and prosocial behaviors in preschool children with behavior problems , 1995, Development and Psychopathology.

[7]  Richard J Davidson,et al.  Empathy is associated with dynamic change in prefrontal brain electrical activity during positive emotion in children. , 2009, Child development.

[8]  Filippo Aureli,et al.  Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[9]  Brent Waters,et al.  The early development of empathy: Self-regulation and individual differences in the first year , 1990 .

[10]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: early signs of concern for others. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.

[11]  Ross A. Thompson,et al.  Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations , 2007 .

[12]  L. Korteweg,et al.  Socially deprived rhesus macaques fail to reconcile: do they not attempt or not accept reconciliation? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern among chimpanzees , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  H. Harlow Total Social Isolation: Effects on Macaque Monkey Behavior. , 1965, Science.

[15]  Sergey Koren,et al.  The bonobo genome compared with the chimpanzee and human genomes , 2012, Nature.

[16]  J. Van Hooff,et al.  Differences in Subadult Behaviors of Chimpanzees Living in Peer Groups and in a Family Group , 1997, International Journal of Primatology.

[17]  S. Preston,et al.  Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[18]  F. Waal,et al.  Reconciliation and Redirected Affection in Rhesus Monkeys , 1983 .

[19]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Empathy-Related Responding in Children , 2013 .

[20]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Concern for Others in the First Year of Life: Theory, Evidence, and Avenues for Research , 2013 .

[21]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Temperament and social behavior in childhood , 1994 .

[22]  T. Singer,et al.  The empathic brain: how, when and why? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  T. Furuichi Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society , 2011, Evolutionary anthropology.

[24]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  The development of empathy in twins. , 1992 .

[25]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: a longitudinal study. , 2006, Emotion.

[26]  James R. Anderson,et al.  Delay of gratification in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.

[27]  Daniel Stahl,et al.  Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda,et al.  Child psychology : a handbook of contemporary issues , 2005 .

[29]  F B de Waal,et al.  Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[30]  Delay of Gratification and Delay Maintenance by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta) , 2007, The Journal of general psychology.

[31]  P. Plotsky,et al.  Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models , 2001, Development and Psychopathology.

[32]  Richard A. Fabes,et al.  The relations of children's dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. , 1996 .

[33]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Contemporaneous and Longitudinal Relations of Dispositional Sympathy to Emotionality, Regulation, and Social Functioning , 1999 .

[34]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Bonobos Respond to Distress in Others: Consolation across the Age Spectrum , 2013, PloS one.

[35]  Shunji Goto,et al.  Decrease in nasal temperature of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in negative emotional state , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  The Origins of Empathy and Altruism , 1985 .

[37]  Gene P. Sackett,et al.  Nursery Rearing of Nonhuman Primates in the 21st Century , 2006 .

[38]  F. D. de Waal Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  C. Nelson The Effects of Early Adversity on Neurobehavioral Development , 2000 .

[40]  Frans Lanting,et al.  Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape , 1997 .

[41]  M. D. De Bellis The Psychobiology of Neglect , 2005, Child maltreatment.

[42]  E. Sterck,et al.  Social competence is reduced in socially deprived rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.

[43]  Jeanne Altmann,et al.  Social Bonds of Female Baboons Enhance Infant Survival , 2003, Science.

[44]  M. Rutter,et al.  Do the effects of early severe deprivation on cognition persist into early adolescence? Findings from the English and Romanian adoptees study. , 2006, Child development.

[45]  J. Kagan,et al.  The Long Shadow of Temperament , 2004 .

[46]  K. Bard,et al.  Enhancement of attachment and cognitive development of young nursery-reared chimpanzees in responsive versus standard care. , 2009, Developmental psychobiology.

[47]  Brian Hare,et al.  Psychological Health of Orphan Bonobos and Chimpanzees in African Sanctuaries , 2011, PloS one.

[48]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity , 2007, Biology Letters.

[49]  B. Tizard,et al.  IQ and behavioural adjustment of ex-institutional adolescents. , 1989, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[50]  P. Fisher,et al.  Predictors of disruptive behavior, developmental delays, anxiety, and affective symptomatology among institutionally reared romanian children. , 2004, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[51]  Matthew F Glasser,et al.  Differences between chimpanzees and bonobos in neural systems supporting social cognition. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[52]  L. Andrews Relation of the , 2011 .

[53]  K. Kotrschal,et al.  Heart rate modulation in bystanding geese watching social and non-social events , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.