Number Meaning and Number Grammar in English and Spanish.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] William F. Brewer,et al. Memory for goal-directed events , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[2] Eleanor Rosch Heider,et al. The structure of the color space in naming and memory for two languages , 1972 .
[3] Brian Butterworth,et al. Constructing Subject-Verb Agreement in Speech: The Role of Semantic and Morphological Factors , 1995 .
[4] John D. Bransford,et al. The abstraction of linguistic ideas , 1971 .
[5] David P. Vinson,et al. Does the grammatical count/mass distinction affect semantic representations? Evidence from experiments in English and Japanese , 2010 .
[6] Peggy Li,et al. Of substance: The nature of language effects on entity construal , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[7] Terry Kit-fong Au. Counterfactuals: In reply to Alfred Bloom , 1984, Cognition.
[8] E. Spelke,et al. Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .
[9] P. Kay,et al. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution , 1973 .
[10] Kathryn Bock,et al. The role of morphology in subject–verb number agreement: A comparison of Mexican and Dominican Spanish , 2012 .
[11] David Barner,et al. Children's Early Understanding of Mass-Count Syntax: Individuation, Lexical Content, and the Number Asymmetry Hypothesis , 2006 .
[12] S. Levinson,et al. Rethinking Linguistic Relativity , 1991, Current Anthropology.
[13] A. Paivio,et al. Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[14] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production. , 2002 .
[15] M. Potter,et al. Regeneration in the short-term recall of sentences , 1990 .
[16] David P. Vinson,et al. Is "Count" and "Mass" Information Available When the Noun Is Not? An Investigation of Tip of the Tongue States and Anomia , 1999 .
[17] A. Bloom,et al. The Linguistic Shaping of Thought: A Study in the Impact of Language on Thinking in China and the West , 1981 .
[18] J. Lucy,et al. Grammatical categories and cognition: References , 1992 .
[19] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[20] WILLIAM MARSLEN-WILSON,et al. Linguistic Structure and Speech Shadowing at Very Short Latencies , 1973, Nature.
[21] K. Bock,et al. The Ties That Bind: Creating Number Agreement in Speech , 1999 .
[22] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall , 1972 .
[23] D. Slobin. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .
[24] D. Slobin. Thinking for Speaking , 1987 .
[25] Brian Butterworth,et al. Subject-verb agreement in Spanish and English: Differences in the role of conceptual constraints , 1996, Cognition.
[26] Mutsurni Irnai,et al. A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning : universal ontology and linguistic influence , 1994 .
[27] Kathryn Bock,et al. Agreement and attraction in Russian , 2008 .
[28] Heidi Lorimor. Conjunctions and Grammatical Agreement , 2007 .
[29] J. Lucy. Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis , 1992 .
[30] R. Hartsuiker,et al. The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.
[31] H. H. Clark. The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .
[32] Roger S. Brown. Reference in memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg , 1976, Cognition.
[33] Ivan A. Sag,et al. Book Reviews: Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar and German in Head-driven Phrase-structure Grammar , 1996, CL.
[34] J. Carroll,et al. Language, Thought and Reality , 1957 .
[35] J. Snedeker,et al. Quantity judgments and individuation: evidence that mass nouns count , 2005, Cognition.
[36] J. Davidoff,et al. Color categories are not universal: replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[37] K. Bock,et al. Producing number agreement: How pronouns equal verbs , 2004 .
[38] David Barner,et al. Language, thought, and real nouns , 2009, Cognition.
[39] David Barner,et al. Events and the ontology of individuals: Verbs as a source of individuating mass and count nouns , 2008, Cognition.
[40] Peggy Li,et al. Words as Windows to Thought , 2010, Current directions in psychological science.
[41] Kathryn Bock,et al. Making syntax of sense: number agreement in sentence production. , 2005, Psychological review.
[42] Matthew J. Rambert,et al. Number Agreement in British and American English: Disagreeing to Agree Collectively , 2006 .
[43] T. Berg. The resolution of number conflicts in English and German agreement patterns , 1998 .
[44] K. Allan,et al. Nouns and Countability , 1980 .
[45] T. Newcomb,et al. Readings in Social Psychology , 1948, Teachers College Record.
[46] Kathleen M. Eberhard,et al. The Accessibility of Conceptual Number to the Processes of Subject–Verb Agreement in English☆☆☆ , 1999 .
[47] K. Bock,et al. Broken agreement , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[48] M. MacDonald,et al. Conflicting cues and competition in subject-verb agreement , 2003 .
[49] T. Au,et al. Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis revisited , 1983, Cognition.
[50] K. Bock,et al. Some Attractions of Verb Agreement , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[51] K Bock,et al. Language production: Methods and methodologies , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[52] Kathryn Bock,et al. Notional number agreement in English , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[53] L. M. Mcpherson,et al. A little goes a long way: evidence for a perceptual basis of learning for the noun categories COUNT and Mass , 1991, Journal of Child Language.
[54] J. Carroll,et al. The function of language classification in behavior , 1958 .
[55] Mutsumi Imai,et al. Separating the chaff from the oats: Evidence for a conceptual distinction between count noun and mass noun aggregates , 2004 .
[56] P. Kay,et al. Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[57] N. Soja. Inferences about the meanings of nouns: The relationship between perception and syntax , 1992 .
[58] Adrian Staub,et al. Response time distributional evidence for distinct varieties of number attraction , 2010, Cognition.
[59] Avital Deutsch,et al. Conflicting Cues and Competition between Notional and Grammatical Factors in Producing Number and Gender Agreement: Evidence from Hebrew. , 2009 .
[60] S. Engel. Thought and Language , 1964 .
[61] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. One or more labels on the bottles? Notional concord in Dutch and French , 1996 .
[62] Willard Van Orman Quine,et al. Word and Object , 1960 .
[63] Gary S. Dell,et al. Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect , 2008, Cognition.
[64] A. Staub. On the interpretation of the number attraction effect: Response time evidence. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.
[65] P. Kay,et al. Language, thought and color: recent developments , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[66] E. Lenneberg,et al. A study in language and cognition. , 1954, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[67] Judith N. Levi,et al. Papers from the eighth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society : April 14-16, 1972 , 1972 .