Effects of aging on the novelty P3 during attend and ignore oddball tasks.

The effects of attention were assessed on novelty P3 amplitude and scalp distribution elicited by environmental sounds in young and elderly volunteers who participated in either actively attended or ignored oddball conditions. For the young, novelty P3 amplitude decreased with time on task during both attend and ignore sequences. Amplitude decrements were greatest at frontal sites during the attend condition, but at all sites during the ignore condition. A reliable amplitude decrement was not observed for the elderly in either the attend or ignore oddball series. The data suggest that attention differentially activates multiple generators that contribute to scalp-recorded novelty P3 activity. The lack of novelty P3 habituation seen in the elderly is consistent with changes in frontal lobe function as age increases.

[1]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Polich Habituation of P300 from auditory stimuli , 1989, Psychobiology.

[3]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  The effect of attention on the P300 deflection elicited by novel sounds , 1995 .

[6]  S. Tipper Less attentional selectivity as a result of declining inhibition in older adults , 1991 .

[7]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Memory and aging: An event-related brain potential perspective , 1995 .

[8]  M S Albert,et al.  Differences in abstraction ability with age. , 1990, Psychology and aging.

[9]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Naming norms for brief environmental sounds: effects of age and dementia. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[10]  John Polich,et al.  P3(00) habituation from auditory and visual stimuli , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[12]  C. Gonnella The Working Brain: An Introduction to Neuropsychology , 1976 .

[13]  E. John,et al.  Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.

[14]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  David Friedman,et al.  Effect of Sound Familiarity on the Event-Related Potentials Elicited by Novel Environmental Sounds , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  R. Mayeux,et al.  Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson disease , 1981, Neurology.

[17]  J. Goodwin,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sort Test performance in a healthy elderly population. , 1987, Journal of gerontology.

[18]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  The neuropsychology of memory and aging. , 1992 .

[19]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  A physiological framework for perceptual and cognitive changes in aging. , 1992 .

[20]  E Courchesne,et al.  Changes in P3 waves with event repetition: long-term effects on scalp distribution and amplitude. , 1978, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  W. Ritter,et al.  Mismatch negativity during attend and ignore conditions in Alzheimer's disease , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  D Friedman,et al.  Item and source memory: differential age effects revealed by event‐related potentials , 1997, Neuroreport.

[23]  P. Satz,et al.  An abbreviation of the WAIS for clinical use. , 1962, Journal of clinical psychology.

[24]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[25]  D Friedman,et al.  Age-related changes in scalp topography to novel and target stimuli. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[26]  D. Friedman,et al.  An ERP developmental study of repetition priming by auditory novel stimuli. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[27]  D. Woods,et al.  Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: an event-related brain potential study. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[29]  P. Goldman-Rakic Working memory and the mind. , 1992, Scientific American.

[30]  Gwenn W. Gröndal,et al.  Meta-analytic procedures for social research , 1993 .

[31]  J. Teresi,et al.  The SHORT-CARE: an efficient instrument for the assessment of depression, dementia and disability. , 1984, Journal of gerontology.

[32]  Fergus I M Craik,et al.  Relations between source amnesia and frontal lobe functioning in older adults. , 1990, Psychology and aging.

[33]  D. Friedman,et al.  A developmental study of the effect of temporal order on the ERPs elicited by novel environmental sounds. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[34]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[35]  R. Johnson A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[36]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  P. Badia,et al.  Habituation of P300 to target stimuli , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[38]  A. Kok,et al.  Arousal and effort: a review and theoretical synthesis of studies of age-related changes in event-related potentials. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[39]  P. Badia,et al.  Time of day and semantic category effects on late components of the visual ERP , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[40]  R. Knight,et al.  Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3 , 1989, Brain Research.

[41]  J. Ford,et al.  Elderly men and women are less responsive to startling noises: N1, P3 and blink evidence , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[42]  D L Woods,et al.  Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after dorsolateral prefrontal lesions , 1986, Neurology.

[43]  F. Craik,et al.  Comparison of older people and patients with frontal lesions: evidence from world list learning. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[44]  L Hasher,et al.  Aging and suppression: memory for previously relevant information. , 1991, Psychology and aging.

[45]  Marc W Howard,et al.  Dissociations Between Recency and Association in the Temporal Context Model : A Description of the Mnemonic Deficit Observed in Cognitive Aging , 2022 .

[46]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  R. Knight Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection , 1996, Nature.

[48]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  AN AGE-DECREMENT IN THE ABILITY TO IGNORE IRRELEVANT INFORMATION. , 1965, Journal of gerontology.