Are frequency-induced transitions in rhythmic coordination mediated by a drop in amplitude?

Abstract. The coordination of rhythmic movements is characterized by attraction to stable modes as well as by loss of stability due to the manipulation of external control parameters. For isochronous coordination between two oscillating components, frequency-induced transitions from antiphase to inphase coordination are frequently observed. Such transitions have been understood on the basis of a dynamical model, the HKB model, consisting of both a potential function for relative phase and a description of the oscillating limbs in terms of nonlinearly coupled limit cycle oscillators. According to the latter aspect of this model, the loss of stability of the antiphase pattern, which precedes the transition to the inphase pattern, is mediated by the decrease in movement amplitude that occurs when the movement frequency is scaled up. This amplitude-based transition mechanism was examined experimentally in the context of a unimanual tracking task. Subjects were instructed to maintain a prescribed amplitude, while tracking an oscillating visual stimulus in either the inphase or the antiphase mode. Three different movement amplitudes were used to examine the prediction that larger amplitudes lead to more stable coordination. When the frequency of oscillation was gradually increased, transitions from antiphase to inphase coordination were observed in the majority of the trials, despite constant or sometimes even slightly increasing amplitudes. No significant effects of amplitude on pattern stability, as indicated by the variability of relative phase and by the critical frequency, were observed. To the extent that these findings can be generalized beyond the present task domain, they suggest that frequency-induced transitions in coordinated rhythmic movements may not be mediated by a drop in amplitude and that alternative directions in modeling may have to be considered.

[1]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[2]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[3]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[4]  R. H. Wimmers,et al.  Phase transitions in rhythmic tracking movements: A case of unilateral coupling , 1992 .

[5]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990 .

[6]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Effects of noise on the phase dynamics of nonlinear oscillators , 1998 .

[7]  P. V. Wieringen,et al.  Limit cycle properties of rhythmic forearm movements. , 1996 .

[8]  R. Chua,et al.  Asymmetries in Coupling Dynamics of Perception and Action. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. , 1987 .

[10]  M. Hildebrand The Adaptive Significance of Tetrapod Gait Selection , 1980 .

[11]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[12]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Dynamical Models of Movement Coordination , 1995 .

[13]  S. Grillner,et al.  The adaptation to speed in human locomotion , 1979, Brain Research.

[14]  J. Kelso The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics , 1994 .

[15]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Distinguishing between the effects of frequency and amplitude on interlimb coupling in tapping a 2:3 polyrhythm , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[17]  J. Kelso,et al.  Action-Perception as a Pattern Formation Process , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[18]  D. C. Shapiro,et al.  Evidence for generalized motor programs using gait pattern analysis. , 1981, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping , 1996 .

[20]  D. F. Hoyt,et al.  Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses , 1981, Nature.

[21]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[23]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination , 1994 .

[24]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical slowing down and switching time , 1987 .

[25]  J. Kelso,et al.  Manipulating symmetry in the coordination dynamics of human movement. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  K. Mardia Statistics of Directional Data , 1972 .

[27]  J A Kelso,et al.  A theoretical note on models of interlimb coordination. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  H. Haken,et al.  Phase-locked modes, phase transitions and component oscillators in biological motion , 1987 .

[29]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. , 1988, Psychological review.

[30]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  M. Turvey,et al.  Coupling dynamics in interlimb coordination. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.