Modeling Theory: Some Traditions, Trends, and Disputes

Publisher Summary This chapter discusses the limitations ascribed to modeling in the experimental methodology rather than in the phenomenon itself. Several experiments are conducted requiring a higher-order form of modeling. These studies use a paradigm in which persons display a consistent style of behavior to diverse stimuli; tests for generalized modeling effects are subsequently conducted by different experimenters in different social settings with the models absent and with different stimulus items. The results disclose that observers respond to new stimulus situations in a manner consistent with the models' dispositions even though subjects had never observed the models responding to these particular stimuli. It is shown that modeling influences effect broader and more complex psychological changes than is commonly assumed. With few exceptions, the experiments conducted in the laboratory use a nonresponse acquisition procedure in which a person simply observes a model's behavior; however, otherwise the person exhibits neither overt responses nor is administered any reinforcing stimuli during the acquisition period. As in this mode of response acquisition, observers can learn the modeled responses only in representational forms, the modeling paradigm provides an interesting means of studying symbolic processes and their performance guiding functions.

[1]  Imitation: Effects of movement and static events , 1969 .

[2]  M. Gerst,et al.  Symbolic Coding Processes in Observational Learning. , 1971 .

[3]  W. Steinman The social control of generalized imitation. , 1970, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[4]  A. Bandura,et al.  INFLUENCE OF MODEL'S REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES ON THE ACQUISITION OF IMITATIVE RESPONSES. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[5]  A. Luchins,et al.  Learning A Complex Ritualized Social Role , 1966 .

[6]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[7]  A. Bandura,et al.  INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL REINFORCEMENT AND THE BEHAVIOR OF MODELS IN SHAPING CHILDREN'S MORAL JUDGMENTS. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[8]  C. Warden,et al.  Imitative Behavior in Cebus and Rhesus Monkeys , 1940 .

[9]  A. Bandura,et al.  Modification of syntactic style. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  B. Foss Mimicry in Mynas (Gracula Religiosa): A Test of Mowrer's Theory , 1964, Psychological Modeling.

[11]  W. Steinman Generalized imitation and the discrimination hypothesis , 1970 .

[12]  M. Branch,et al.  Comparison of the relative effectiveness of instructions, modeling, and reinforcement procedures for inducing behavior change. , 1969 .

[13]  Justin Aronfreed,et al.  CHAPTER THREE – THE CONCEPT OF INTERNALIZATION , 1968 .

[14]  D. Baer,et al.  Reinforcement control of generalized imitation in young children , 1964 .

[15]  A. Bandura,et al.  Conditions Governing Nonreinforced Imitation. , 1971 .

[16]  D M Baer,et al.  The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model. , 1967, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  A. Bandura,et al.  A COMPARATIVE TEST OF THE STATUS ENVY, SOCIAL POWER, AND SECONDARY REINFORCEMENT THEORIES OF IDENTIFICATORY LEARNING. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  Albert Bandura,et al.  Observational learning as a function of symbolization and incentive set. , 1966 .

[19]  G R Patterson,et al.  Assertive behavior in children: a step toward a theory of aggression. , 1967, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[20]  A. Bandura,et al.  Modification of self-imposed delay of reward through exposure to live and symbolic models. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.