Workaholism as Discretionary Time Investment at Work: An Experience‐Sampling Study

Adopting an operational definition of workaholism as discretionary investment of considerable time at work, the purpose of the present study was to test hypotheses regarding the cognitive aspect of workaholism, as well as the positive and negative/addictive views of this construct. The study employed an experience-sampling method (ESM), using a sample of 65 full-time employees who completed the ESM forms at four random times during the day for one week. Results indicated that workaholism was associated with continued cognitive engagement with work, accompanied by a preference for work over leisure activity and higher positive affect during work activity than during leisure activity. No significant differences were found between workaholics and non-workaholics with regard to the likelihood of performing work-related activities during leisure activity, or in the levels of physical discomfort and negative affect during the weekend. These results highlight the utility of an operational framework for studying the variety of workaholism correlates. Theoretical and applied implications are discussed. A partir d’une definition operationnelle de l’addiction au travail comme etant un investissement sans controle d’un temps considerable consacre au travail, cette recherche s’est donne comme objectif de mettre a l’epreuve des hypotheses relatives a la dimension cognitive de l’addiction au travail, ainsi que les aspects positifs et negatifs (addictifs) de ce concept. Cette recherche a exploite la methode de l’echantillonnage des experiences (ESM) a partir d’un echantillon compose de 65 salaries a plein-temps qui ont rempli la feuille de recueil des donnees quatre fois par jour au hasard pendant une semaine. Les resultats ont montre que l’addiction au travail etait liee a un engagement cognitif permanent vis-a-vis du travail, associea une plus forte attirance pour le travail que pour les loisirs et a un etat affectif plus fortement positif lors du travail qu’a l’occasion des activites de loisirs. On n’a pas trouve de differences significatives entre les drogues du travail et les autres en ce qui concerne la probabilite de realiser des activites relevant du travail pendant les loisirs, ou dans le niveau d’inconfort physique et le poids des affects negatifs pendant le week-end. Ces resultats montrent l’utilite d’un schema operationnel dans l’etude de la diversite des phenomenes relies a l’addiction au travail. Les implications theoriques et pratiques sont examinees.

[1]  B. Robinson The Work Addiction Risk Test: Development of a Tentative Measure of Workaholism , 1999, Perceptual and motor skills.

[2]  Nigel V. Marsh,et al.  Understanding Workaholism: Data Synthesis, Theoretical Critique, and Future Design Strategies , 2001 .

[3]  Y. Fried,et al.  The effects of hours of work on health: A meta-analytic review. , 1997 .

[4]  B. Robinson,et al.  Measuring Workaholism: Content Validity of the Work Addiction Risk Test , 1995, Psychological reports.

[5]  R. Burke,et al.  Workaholism among Norwegian managers , 2004 .

[6]  B. Schneider THE PEOPLE MAKE THE PLACE , 1987 .

[7]  M. Bellas,et al.  The Overworked American or the Time Bind? , 2001 .

[8]  R. Burke Spence and Robbins' Measures of Workaholism Components: Test-Retest Stability , 2001, Psychological reports.

[9]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[10]  M. Wakabayashi,et al.  Effects of economic environmental changes on job demands and workaholism in Japan , 2004 .

[11]  S. Zeger,et al.  Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models , 1986 .

[12]  J. Spence,et al.  Workaholism: definition, measurement, and preliminary results. , 1992, Journal of personality assessment.

[13]  Arthur P. Brief,et al.  The economic instrumentality of work: An examination of the moderating effects of financial requirements and sex on the pay-life satisfaction relationship☆ , 1990 .

[14]  R. Burke Workaholism in Organizations: Measurement Validation and Replication , 1999 .

[15]  M. Csíkszentmihályi,et al.  Validity and Reliability of the Experience‐Sampling Method , 1987, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[16]  D. Watson,et al.  Mood and the mundane: relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  N. Marsh,et al.  A multifaceted validation study of Spence and Robbins' (1992) Workaholism Battery , 2002 .

[18]  Y. Liu,et al.  Overtime work, insufficient sleep, and risk of non-fatal acute myocardial infarction in Japanese men , 2002, Occupational and environmental medicine.

[19]  W. Ickes,et al.  Personality influences on the choice of situations , 1997 .

[20]  L. Clark,et al.  Symptoms and traits of personality disorder: Two new methods for their assessment. , 1993 .

[21]  M. Zickar,et al.  A cluster analysis investigation of workaholism as a syndrome. , 2006, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[22]  Phyllis Moen,et al.  Understanding the Time-Squeeze , 2001 .

[23]  Luc Goossens,et al.  Concurrent and Predictive Validity of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire in a Sample of European Freshman Students , 2002 .

[24]  B. Robinson Concurrent Validity of the Work Addiction Risk Test as a Measure of Workaholism , 1996, Psychological reports.

[25]  P. Salomone,et al.  Understanding Workaholism: A Review of Causes and Counseling Approaches. , 1994 .

[26]  M. van der Hulst,et al.  Long workhours and health. , 2003 .

[27]  G. Porter Organizational impact of workaholism: suggestions for researching the negative outcomes of excessive work. , 1996, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[28]  Kevin J. Williams,et al.  USING SIGNAL‐CONTINGENT EXPERIENCE SAMPLING METHODOLOGY TO STUDY WORK IN THE FIELD: A DISCUSSION AND ILLUSTRATION EXAMINING TASK PERCEPTIONS AND MOOD , 1993 .

[29]  Marcia P. Miceli,et al.  An Exploration of the Meaning and Consequences of Workaholism , 1997 .

[30]  Lawrence A. Pervin,et al.  Persons, Situations, Interactions: The History of a Controversy and a Discussion of Theoretical Models , 1989 .

[31]  Benjamin Schneider,et al.  The ASA framework: An update. , 1995 .

[32]  B. Robinson,et al.  Validity of the Work Addiction Risk Test , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[33]  J. B. Erickson,et al.  The impact of overtime and long work hours on occupational injuries and illnesses: new evidence from the United States , 2005, Occupational and Environmental Medicine.

[34]  R. Rosenthal,et al.  Self-reported withdrawal symptoms and pathological gambling. , 1992 .

[35]  Itzhak Harpaz,et al.  Workaholism: Its Definition and Nature , 2003 .

[36]  S. Eysenck,et al.  A further investigation into the personality of drug addicts in treatment. , 1980, British journal of addiction.

[37]  J. Jacobs,et al.  Globalization, Work Hours, And The Care Deficit Among Stockbrokers , 2003, Global Dimensions of Gender and Carework.

[38]  T. Naughton A Conceptual View of Workaholism and Implications for Career Counseling and Research. , 1987 .

[39]  Linda K. Stroh,et al.  Working 61 plus hours a week: why do managers do it? , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[40]  E. Diener,et al.  Situation selection as a moderator of response consistency and stability. , 1986 .

[41]  Ed Diener,et al.  A goal-affect analysis of everyday situational choices☆ , 1986 .