An experimental study of a virtual reality counselling paradigm using embodied self-dialogue

When faced with a personal problem people typically give better advice to others than to themselves. A previous study showed how it is possible to enact internal dialogue in virtual reality (VR) through participants alternately occupying two different virtual bodies – one representing themselves and the other Sigmund Freud. They could maintain a self-conversation by explaining their problem to the virtual Freud and then from the embodied perspective of Freud see and hear the explanation by their virtual doppelganger, and then give some advice. Alternating between the two bodies they could maintain a self-dialogue, as if between two different people. Here we show that the process of alternating between their own and the Freud body is important for successful psychological outcomes. An experiment was carried out with 58 people, 29 in the body swapping Self-Conversation condition and 29 in a condition where they only spoke to a Scripted Freud character. The results showed that the Self-Conversation method results in a greater perception of change and help compared to the Scripted. We compare this method with the distancing paradigm where participants imagine resolving a problem from a first or third person perspective. We consider the method as a possible strategy for self-counselling.

[1]  Mel Slater,et al.  Virtual Embodiment of White People in a Black Virtual Body Leads to a Sustained Reduction in Their Implicit Racial Bias , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[2]  Chad E. Forbes,et al.  Self-distancing improves interpersonal perceptions and behavior by decreasing medial prefrontal cortex activity during the provision of criticism , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[3]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership causes illusory self-attribution of speaking and influences subsequent real speaking , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Slater,et al.  Virtual reality in the assessment, understanding, and treatment of mental health disorders , 2017, Psychological Medicine.

[5]  Mel Slater,et al.  Automated psychological therapy using immersive virtual reality for treatment of fear of heights: a single-blind, parallel-group, randomised controlled trial , 2018, The lancet. Psychiatry.

[6]  D. Sloan,et al.  Self-Distancing From Trauma Memories Reduces Physiological but Not Subjective Emotional Reactivity Among Veterans With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder , 2015, Clinical psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[7]  M. Slater,et al.  Measuring the Effects through Time of the Influence of Visuomotor and Visuotactile Synchronous Stimulation on a Virtual Body Ownership Illusion , 2014, Perception.

[8]  Pasco Fearon,et al.  Embodying self-compassion within virtual reality and its effects on patients with depression , 2016, BJPsych open.

[9]  Mel Slater,et al.  Drumming in Immersive Virtual Reality: The Body Shapes the Way We Play , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[10]  Mel Slater,et al.  Virtually Being Einstein Results in an Improvement in Cognitive Task Performance and a Decrease in Age Bias , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[11]  Liyang Sai,et al.  Self-Distancing Reduces Probability-Weighting Biases , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[12]  P. Deldin,et al.  "Asking why" from a distance: its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. , 2012, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[13]  Mel Slater,et al.  Conversations between self and self as Sigmund Freud—A virtual body ownership paradigm for self counselling , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[14]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[15]  Tim P. Moran,et al.  Third-person self-talk facilitates emotion regulation without engaging cognitive control: Converging evidence from ERP and fMRI , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[16]  R. Elliott,et al.  Qualitative Change Process Research on Psychotherapy: Alternative Strategies. , 2001 .

[17]  A. Beck,et al.  Cognitive behavior therapy : basics and beyond , 2011 .

[18]  Joan Llobera,et al.  The relationship between virtual body ownership and temperature sensitivity , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[19]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Changing bodies changes minds: owning another body affects social cognition , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  First Person Experience of Body Transfer in Virtual Reality , 2010, PloS one.

[21]  John A. King,et al.  Embodying Compassion: A Virtual Reality Paradigm for Overcoming Excessive Self-Criticism , 2014, PloS one.

[22]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Boosting wisdom: distance from the self enhances wise reasoning, attitudes, and behavior. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  A. Ellis,et al.  Handbook of Rational-Emotive Therapy , 1986 .

[24]  Tabitha C. Peck,et al.  Putting yourself in the skin of a black avatar reduces implicit racial bias , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[25]  Justin M. Harris,et al.  If I Were You: Perceptual Illusion of Body Swapping , 2008, PloS one.

[26]  M. Slater,et al.  Embodiment in a Child-Like Talking Virtual Body Influences Object Size Perception, Self-Identification, and Subsequent Real Speaking , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[27]  Sanda Dolcos,et al.  The inner speech of behavioral regulation: Intentions and task performance strengthen when you talk to yourself as a You , 2014 .

[28]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  The body fades away: investigating the effects of transparency of an embodied virtual body on pain threshold and body ownership , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[29]  Béatrice S. Hasler,et al.  Virtual race transformation reverses racial in-group bias , 2017, PloS one.

[30]  Tim P. Moran,et al.  Third-Person Self-Talk Reduces Ebola Worry and Risk Perception by Enhancing Rational Thinking. , 2017, Applied psychology. Health and well-being.

[31]  Manos Tsakiris,et al.  Experiencing ownership over a dark-skinned body reduces implicit racial bias , 2013, Cognition.

[32]  B. Wampold,et al.  How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update , 2015, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[33]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Exploring Solomon’s Paradox: Self-Distancing Eliminates the Self-Other Asymmetry in Wise Reasoning About Close Relationships in Younger and Older Adults , 2014, Psychological science.

[34]  M. Slater,et al.  Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Self-Distancing: Theory, Research, and Current Directions , 2017 .

[36]  Alex C. Huynh,et al.  The Wisdom in Virtue: Pursuit of Virtue Predicts Wise Reasoning About Personal Conflicts , 2017, Psychological science.

[37]  Mel Slater,et al.  Seeing an Embodied Virtual Hand is Analgesic Contingent on Colocation. , 2017, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[38]  M. Seligman,et al.  The effectiveness of psychotherapy. The Consumer Reports study. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[39]  G. Andrews,et al.  Validity of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) psychosis module in a psychiatric setting. , 1998, Journal of psychiatric research.